全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Similarities and Differences between Child and Adult L2 Learners in the Acquisition of English Reflexives

DOI: 10.4236/ojml.2019.96038, PP. 508-539

Keywords: Second Language Acquisition, Reflexive Binding, Syntax, Fundamental Difference Hypothesis, Domain-by-Age Model, Universal Grammar

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

This paper reports on an experimental study which tested the claims of the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis and the Domain-by-Age-Model by comparing the performances of Arabic- and Chinese-speaking child and adult L2 learners in the acquisition of English reflexives. Whilst English and Arabic allow only local binding of reflexives, Chinese allows local and long-distance binding of reflexives. 60 L2 learners were given a word-based mean length of utterance proficiency test to confirm their proficiency level and, then, they were divided into six groups: Arabic-speaking children (n = 15); Arabic- speaking adults (n = 15); Chinese-speaking children (n = 15); Chinese- speaking adults (n = 15); an L1-English child (mean age 9.60) control group (n = 15); and an L1-English adult control group (n = 15). L2 learners’ interpretation of English reflexives was elicited through a 48-item Simon-Says- game. The results showed significant differences between the performance, of the L2 groups, and that of native speakers; yet, the majority of L2 learners were close to an 83.33% acquisition threshold. Also, by showing no significant difference between the child and adult L2 groups, the results indicated support for the Domain-by-Age-Model. As for the syntactic difference between reflexives and pronouns, L2 learners did not differentiate between them, scoring higher in reflexives.

References

[1]  Al Kafri, A. (2008). Interpretation of English Reflexives by Non-Native Speakers (Arab and Chinese). Colchester: Essex University.
[2]  Bennett, S. (1994). Interpretation of English Reflexives by Adolescent Speakers of Serbo- Croatian. Second Language Research, 10, 125-156.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839401000202
[3]  Bley-Vroman, R. (1990). The Logical Problem of Foreign Language Learning. Linguistic Analysis, 20, 3-49.
[4]  Bley-Vroman, R. (2009). The Evolving Context of the Fundamental Difference Hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 31, 175-198.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263109090275
[5]  Blom, E. (2008). Testing the Domain-by-Age Model Inflection and Placement of Dutch Verbs. In B. Haznedar, & E. Gavruseva (Eds.), Current Trends in Child Second Language Acquisition (pp. 271-300). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.46.14blo
[6]  Chien, Y.C., & Wexler, K. (1987). Children’s Acquisition of the Locality Principle for Reflexives and Pronouns. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 26, 1-17.
[7]  Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
https://doi.org/10.21236/AD0616323
[8]  Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.
[9]  Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use. Westport, CT: Praeger.
[10]  Clahsen, H., & Hong, U. (1995). Agreement and Null Subjects in German L2 Development: New Evidence from Reaction-Time Experiments. Second Language Research, 11, 57-87.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839501100103
[11]  Eubank, L. (1993). On the Transfer of Parametric Values in L2 Development. Language Acquisition, 3, 183-208.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0303_1
[12]  Eubank, L. (1994). Optionality and the Initial State in L2 Development. In T. Hoekstra, & B. Schwartz (Eds.), Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar: Papers in Honor of Kenneth Wexler from the 1991 GLOW Workshops (pp. 369-388). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.8.15eub
[13]  Finer, D. L., & Broselow, E. I. (1986). Second Language Acquisition of Reflexive Binding. Proceedings of NELS, 16, 154-168.
[14]  Hirakawa, M. (1990). A Study of the L2 Acquisition of English Reflexives. Second Language Research, 6, 60-85.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839000600103
[15]  Kremers, J. (1997). How Arabs Speak to Each Other about Themselves: A Study of NAFS and Bad in Modern Standard Arabic. Nijmegen: Katholieke Universiteit.
[16]  Lee, D., & Schachter, J. (1997). Sensitive Period Effects in Binding Theory. Language Acquisition, 6, 333-362.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0604_3
[17]  Lee, S. (2005). Development in the L2 Acquisition of English Reflexives by Korean Adults and Children. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii.
[18]  MacLaughlin, D. (1998). The Acquisition of the Morphosyntax of English Reflexives by Non-Native Speakers. In M.L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and Its Interfaces in Second Language Knowledge (pp. 195-226). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.19.09mac
[19]  Osman, M. H. (1990). Bound Anaphora in Egyptian Arabic. In M. Eid (Ed.), Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics I (pp. 156-171). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.63.09osm
[20]  Progovac, L. (1993). Long-Distance Reflexives: Movement-to-INFL versus Relativized Subject. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 755-772.
[21]  Schachter, J. (1990). On the Issue of Completeness in Second Language Acquisition. Second Language Research, 6, 93-124.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839000600201
[22]  Schwartz, B. (2003). Child L2 Acquisition: Paving the Way. In Proceedings of the 27th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (Vol. 1, pp. 26-50).
[23]  Schwartz, B. (2004). Why Child L2 Acquisition? In J. V. Kampen, & S. Baauw (Eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003 (pp. 47-66). Utrecht: Graduate School of Linguistics (LOT).
[24]  Schwartz, B. D. (1992). Testing between UG-Based and Problem-Solving Models of L2A: Developmental Sequence Data. Language Acquisition, 2, 1-19.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0201_1
[25]  Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. (1996). L2 Cognitive States and the Full Transfer/Full Access Model. Second Language Research, 12, 40-72.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839601200103
[26]  Schwartz, B., & Sprouse, R. A. (1994). Word Order and Nominative Case in Non-Native Language Acquisition: A Longitudinal Study of (L1 Turkish). German Interlanguage. In T. Hoekstra, & B. Schwartz (Eds.), Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar: Papersin Honor of Kenneth Wexler from the 1991 GLOW Workshops (pp. 317-368). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.8.14sch
[27]  Slobin, D. (1993). Coding Child Language Data for Cross-Linguistic Analysis. In J. Edwards, & M. Lampert (Eds.), Talking Data: Transcription and Coding in Discourse Research (pp. 207-219). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[28]  Thomas, M. (1989). The Interpretation of English Reflexive Pronouns by Non-Native Speakers. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11, 281-303.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100008147
[29]  Thomas, M. (1995). Acquisition of the Japanese Reflexive Zibun and Movement of Anaphors in Logical Form. Second Language Research, 11, 206-234.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839501100302
[30]  Tsimpli, I., & Roussou, A. (1991). Parameter Resetting in L2 (pp. 149-169). UCL Working Papers in Linguistics.
[31]  Unsworth, S. (2005). Child L2, Adult L2, Child L1: Differences and Similarities. A Study on the Acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling in Dutch. Utrecht: Universiteit Utrecht.
[32]  Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (1994). Direct Access to X-Bar Theory: Evidence from Korean and Turkish Adults Learning German. In T. Hoekstra, & B. Schwartz (Eds.), Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar: Papers in Honor of Kenneth Wexler from the 1991 GLOW Workshops (pp. 265-316). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.8.13vai
[33]  Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (1996). Gradual Development of L2 Phrase Structure. Second Language Research, 12, 7-39.
https://doi.org/10.1177/026765839601200102
[34]  Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (2006). The Roots of Syntax and How They Grow: Organic Grammar, the Basic Variety and Processability Theory. In S. Unsworth, T. Parodi, A. Sorace, & M. Young-Schoten (Eds.), Paths of Development in L1 and L2 Acquisition: In Honor of Bonnie D. Schwartz (pp. 77-106). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.39.05vai
[35]  Vainikka, A., & Young-Scholten, M. (2007). Minimalism vs. Organic Syntax. In S. Karimi, V. Samiian, & W. K. Wilkins (Eds.), Clausal and Phrasal Architecture: Syntactic Derivation and Interpretation (pp. 319-338). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/la.101.16vai
[36]  Wexler, K., & Manzini, R. (1987). Parameters and Learn Ability in Binding Theory. In T. Roeper, & E. Williams (Eds.), Parameter Setting (pp. 41-76). Dordrecht: Reidel D.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-3727-7_3
[37]  Whong-Barr, M., & Schwartz, B. D. (2002). Morphological and Syntactic Transfer in Child L2 Acquisition of the English Dative Alternation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 579-616.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263102004035
[38]  Yip, V., & Tang, J. (1998). Acquisition of English Reflexive Binding by Cantonese Learners: Testing the Positive Transfer Hypothesis. In M.L. Beck (Ed.), Morphology and Its Interfaces in Second Language Knowledge (pp. 165-193). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.19.08yip
[39]  Yuan, B. (1998). Interpretation of Binding and Orientation of the Chinese Reflexive “Ziji” by English and Japanese Speakers. Second Language Research, 14, 324-341.
https://doi.org/10.1191/026765898670904111

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133