Parent level of education makes important contributions to the linguistic development of children (Hoff, 2003). This cross-sectional study, focusing on the link between families’ socioeconomic status (SES) and their language use, aims to investigate whether parents’ levels of SES influence their interaction styles with their children in terms of dialogicality. Dialogicality refers to “the ways in which one speaker’s concrete utterances come into contact with the utterances of another” (Wertsch, 1991: p. 54). Research suggests that when adults give children opportunities of dialogicality and let them become active participants during conversations, i.e. when the interactions are more dialogic, children show greater language gains (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Within this context, this study is an attempt to explore how and to what extent parents from different socioeconomic backgrounds encourage their preschoolers to become more active in their interactions. Ten families differing in SES were audio-taped in their homes for about 15 minutes in the toy play context with their five-year-old children. The transcriptions were coded as exchanges, and then each exchange was coded as an II or IR or IRF pattern according to Sinclair & Coulthard’s spoken discourse model which was developed in 1975. The model, consisting of three-part exchanges: Initiation, Response, and Feedback, known as IRF, was developed through the application of transcripts taken from primary school classroom settings in the 1970’s. Focusing on these parent- preschooler dialogues, this study tried to find out whether families’ interaction styles differentiate depending on their SES; thus, which children were encouraged more to become active speakers before starting their formal education. The results show that SES has a crucial effect upon parents’ dialogicality, that is, high SES parents’ interaction styles are much more dialogic.
References
[1]
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech Genres and Other Late Essays. Austin: University of Texas Press.
[2]
Ball, A. F. (1999). Preservice Teachers’ Perspectives on Literacy and Its Use in Urban Schools. A Vygotskian Perspective on Internal Activity and Teachers Change. In C. Lee, & P. Smagorinsky (Eds.), Vygotskian Perspective on Literacy Research: Constructing Meaning through Collaborative Inquiry (pp. 314-359). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
[3]
Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers’ Influence on Their Children’s Cognitive and Emotional Development: From Toddlers to Pre-K. Applied Developmental Science, 11, 208-223.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888690701762100
[4]
Cengiz, O., & Cakir, H. (2015). An Investigation of Maternal Input within the Framework of Bloom’s Taxonomy of Cognitive Domain. In Zeyrek, D., Sagin Simsek, C.,Atas, U., & Rehbein, J. (Eds.) Ankara Papers in Turkish and Turkic Linguistics (pp. 225-235). Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz Verlag.
[5]
Cengiz, O. (2010). Turk Annelerin Cocuklarina Kitap Okurken Kullandiklari Dilsel Kodlar ve Etkilesim Bicemi. (The Linguistic Codes and the Types of Verbal Interactions of Turkish Mothers in the Context of Shared Book Reading). Unpublished Dissertation. Izmir: Dokuz Eylul University.
[6]
Cengiz, O. (2013). The Language Use of Turkish Mothers in the Context of Shared Book Reading. Journal of Faculty of Letters, 30, 97-114.
[7]
Cengiz, O., & Cakir, H. (2012a). Okuloncesi Cocuklarda Dilbilgisel Eiretileme Kullaniminin Ortaya Cikis Surecinde Annelerinin Etkisi (The Effects of Mothers in the Emergence of Grammatical Metaphor Use in Preschoolers). Journal of Linguistic Research, 2012, 21-39.
[8]
Cengiz, O., & Cakir, H. (2012b). A Study of Fathers’ Language Input with Regard to Bloom’s Taxonomy of Cognitive Domain. Proceedings Book of International Symposium on Language and Communication: Research Trends and Challenges (pp. 565-578). Erzurum: Mega Press.
[9]
Cengiz, O., & Cakir, H. (2016). Turkish Fathers’ Language Use in the Context of Toy Play. European Scientific Journal, 12, 430-447.
[10]
Duursma, E. (2014). The Effects of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Reading to Their Children on Language Outcomes of Children Participating in Early Head Start in the United States. Fathering, 12, 283-302.
[11]
Duursma, E., Pan, B. A., & Raikes, H. (2008). Predictors and Outcomes of Low-Income Fathers’ Reading with Their Toddlers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23, 351-365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.06.001
[12]
Fairclough, N. (2003). Analysing Discourse: Textual Analysis for Social Research. New York: Routledge.
[13]
Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., & Papousek, M. (1989). A Cross-Language Study of Prosodic Modification in Mothers’ and Fathers’ Speech to Preverbal Infants. Journal of Child Language, 19, 477-501.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679
[14]
Fish, M., & Pinkerman, B. (2003). Language Skills in Low-SES Rural Appalachian Children: Normative Development and individual Differences, infancy to Preschool. Applied Developmental Psychology, 23, 539-565.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00141-7
[15]
Golinkoff, R. M., & Ames, G. J. (1979). A Comparison of Fathers’ and Mothers’ Speech with Their Young Children. Child Development, 50, 28-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129037
[16]
Hart. B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co., Inc.
[17]
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[18]
Hladik, E. G., & Edwards, H. T. (1984). A Comparative Analysis of Mother-Father Speech in the Naturalistic Home Environment. Journal ofPsycholinguistic Research, 13, 321-332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01068149
[19]
Hoff, E. (2003). The Specificity of Environmental Influence: Socioeconomic Status Affects Early Vocabulary Development via Maternal Speech. Child Development, 74, 1368-1378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612
[20]
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic Status and Parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of Parenting (pp. 231-252). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
[21]
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The Relation of Birth Order and Socioeconomic Status to Children’s Language Development. Applied Psycholingistics, 19, 603-629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010389
[22]
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of Variability in Children’s Language Growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343-365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002
[23]
Kavanaugh, R. D., & Jirkovsky, A. M. (1982). Parental Speech to Young Children: A Longitudinal Analysis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 297-311.
[24]
Kuntay, A. C., & Ahtam, B. (2004). Annelerin cocukalariyla gecmis hakkindaki konusmalarinin Anne eiitim duzeyiyle iliskisi (Effect of Maternal Education on Turkish Mothers’ Styles of Reminiscing with Their Children). Turkish Journal of Psychology, 19, 19-31.
[25]
Kuntay, A. C., & Slobin, D. I. (2002). Putting Interaction Back into Child Language: Examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication, 6, 5-14.
[26]
Kuntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1995). Nouns and Verbs in Turkish Child Directed Speech. In D. MacLaughlin, & S. McEwen (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development 1 (pp. 323-334). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
[27]
Kuntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1996). Listening to a Turkish Mother: Some Puzzles for Acquisition. In D. I. Slobin, J. Gerhardt, A. Kyratzis, & J. Guo (Ed.), Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language: Essays in Honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp. (pp. 265-286). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
[28]
Kuntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (2001). Discourse Behavior of Lexical Categories in Turkish Child-Directed Speech: Nouns vs. Verbs. In M. Almgren, A. Barrena, M. Ezeizabarrena, I. Idiazabal, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Research on Child Language Acquisition: Proceedings for the 8th Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (pp. 928-946). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
[29]
Kuzucu, Y. (2011). Deiisen babalik rolu ve cocuk gelisimine etkisi (The Changing Role of Fathers and Its Impact on Child Development). Turk Psikolojik Danisma ve Rehberlik Dergisi, 4, 79-91.
[30]
Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of Gender Effects on Parents’ Talk to Their Children: A Meta-Analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 3-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.3
[31]
Liu, J., & Le, T. (2012). A Case Study on College English Classroom Discourse. International Journal of Innovative Interdisciplinary Research, 2, 1-10.
[32]
Lotman, Y. M. (1988). Text within a Text. Soviet Psychology, 26, 32-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/rpo1061-0405260332
[33]
Lyle, S. (2008). Dialogic Teaching: Discussing Theoretical Contexts and Reviewing Evidence from Classroom Practice. Language and Education, 22, 222-240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09500780802152499
[34]
McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (1991). Getting the Story: Parental Styles of Narrative Elicitation and Developing Narrative Skills. In A. McCabe, & C. Petarson (Eds.), Developing Narrative Structure (pp. 217-253). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[35]
McLaughlin, B., White, D., McDevitt, T., & Raskin, R. (1983). Mothers’ and Fathers’ Speech to Their Young Children: Similar or Different? Journal of Child Language, 10, 245-252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900005286
[36]
McRoberts, G. W., & Best, C. T. (1997). Accommodation in Mean f0 during Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions: A Longitudinal Case Study. Journal of Child Language, 24, 719-736.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500099700322X
[37]
Nicholson, S. J. (2014). An Impetus for Change: Classroom Analysis Using Sinclair and Coulthard’s Model of Spoken Discourse. International Journal of Linguistics, 6, 199-219. http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/ijl.v6i2.5464
[38]
Ninio, A. (1980). Picture-Book Reading in Mother-Infant Dyads Belonging to Two Subgroups in Israel. Child Development, 51, 587-590. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129299
[39]
O’Brien, M., & Nagle, K. J. (1987). Parents’ Speech to Toddlers: The Effect of Play Context. Journal of Child Language, 14, 269-279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012927
[40]
O’Connor, C. & Michaels, S. (2007). When Is Dialogue “Dialogic”? Human Development, 50, 275-285.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000106415
[41]
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and Father Language Input to Young Children: Contributions to Later Language Development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27, 571-587.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003
[42]
Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2010). Fathers’ Early Contributions to Children’s Language Development in Families from Low-Income Rural Communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 450-463.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.001
[43]
Pancsofar, N., Vernon-Feagans, L., Odom, E. C., & The Family Life Project Investigators (2013). Work Characteristics and Fathers’ Vocabulary to Infants in African American Families. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 34, 73-81.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2012.11.005
[44]
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1992). Parental Styles of Narrative Elicitation: Effect on Children’s Narrative Structure and Content. First Language, 12, 299-321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014272379201203606
[45]
Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C.S., Brooks-Gunn, J. et al. (2006). Mother-Child Bookreading in Low-income Families: Correlates and Outcomes during the First Three Years of Life. Child Development, 77, 924-953.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x
[46]
Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and Mothers’ Speech in Early Language Development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353-369.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900002671
[47]
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-Directed Speech: Relation to Socioeconomic Status, Knowledge of Child Development and Child Vocabulary Skill. Child Language, 35, 185-205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343
[48]
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A Longitudinal Investigation of the Role of Quantity and Quality of Child Directed Speech in Vocabulary Development. Child Development, 83, 1762-1774. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x
[49]
Sinclair, J., & Coulthard, R. M. (1975). Towards an Analysis of Discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[50]
Stenstrom, A. (1994). An Introduction to Spoken Interaction. London & New York: Longman.
[51]
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J.D., Cabrera, N., & Lamb, M. (2004). Fathers and Mothers at Play with Their 2- and 3- Year-Olds. Contribution to Language and Cognitive Development. Child Development, 75, 1806-1820.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00818.x
Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
[54]
Turkay, F. (2007). Turk Annelerin ad/eylem kullanimlarinin Ba?lam etmeniyle iliskisi. In 21. Dilbilim Kurultayi Bildirileri (pp. 273-276). Mersin: Mersin Universitesi Yayinlari.
[55]
Wells, G. (2007). Semiotic Mediation, Dialogue and the Construction of Knowledge. Human Development, 50, 244-274.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000106414
[56]
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
[57]
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child Development and Emergent Literacy. Child Development, 69, 848-872.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06247.x
[58]
Zeybekoglu, O. (2013). Gunumuz erkeklerin gozunde babalik ve aile (Contemporary Fatherhood and the Family from the Male Perspectives). Mediterranean Journal of Humanities, III, 297-328. http://dx.doi.org/10.13114/MJH/201322486