Drug self-administration procedures have played a critical role in the experimental analysis of psychoactive compounds, such as cocaine, for over 50 years. While there are numerous permutations of this procedure, this paper will specifically focus on choice procedures using concurrent schedules of intravenous drug self-administration. The aims of this paper are to first highlight the evolution of drug choice procedures and then review the subsequent preclinical body of literature utilizing these choice procedures to understand the environmental, pharmacological, and biological determinants of the reinforcing stimulus effects of drugs. A main rationale for this paper is our proposition that choice schedules are underutilized in investigating the reinforcing effects of drugs in assays of drug self-administration. Moreover, we will conclude with potential future directions and unexplored scientific space for the use of drug choice procedures. 1. The Evolution of Drug Choice Procedures Drug self-administration procedures have played a critical role in the experimental analysis of psychoactive compounds, such as cocaine, for more than 50 years. In general, preclinical drug self-administration procedures are utilized for two main scientific purposes. One purpose is in abuse liability testing of psychoactive compounds for potential scheduling as controlled substances by the Drug Enforcement Agency, and there are already excellent reviews on the use of drug self-administration procedures for this purpose, see [1, 2]. The other main purpose of drug self-administration procedures is in understanding the pharmacological, environmental and biological determinants of drug-taking behavior as a model of drug addiction. This paper will focus on the use of concurrent-choice schedules of drug self-administration to address this latter purpose. Although there are numerous permutations of drug self-administration procedures, all use the classic 3-term contingency of operant conditioning to investigate the stimulus properties of drugs [3]. This 3-term contingency can be diagrammed as follows: where designates a discriminative stimulus, designates a response on the part of the organism, and designates a consequent stimulus. The arrows specify the contingency that, in the presence of the discriminative stimulus , performance of the response will result in delivery of the consequent stimulus . As a simple and common example from a preclinical laboratory, a rat implanted with a chronic indwelling catheter might be connected to an infusion pump containing a dose of a
References
[1]
N. A. Ator and R. R. Griffiths, “Principles of drug abuse liability assessment in laboratory animals,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 70, no. 3, pp. S55–S72, 2003.
[2]
L. P. Carter and R. R. Griffiths, “Principles of laboratory assessment of drug abuse liability and implications for clinical development.,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 105, supplement 1, pp. S14–S25, 2009.
[3]
B. F. Skinner, The Behavior of Organisms, Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, NY, USA, 1938.
[4]
C. B. Ferster and B. F. Skinner, Schedules of Reinforcement, Appleton-Century-Croft, New York, NY, USA, 1957.
[5]
J. R. Weeks, “Experimental morphine addiction: method for automatic intravenous injections in unrestrained rats,” Science, vol. 138, no. 3537, pp. 143–144, 1962.
[6]
J. R. Weeks and R. J. Collins, “Factors affecting voluntary morphine intake in self-maintained addicted rats,” Psychopharmacologia, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 267–279, 1964.
[7]
A. Young and S. Herling, “Drugs as reinforcers: studies in laboratory animals,” in Behavioral Analysis of Drug Dependence, S. Goldberg and I. Stolerman, Eds., pp. 9–67, Academic Press, Orlando, Fla, USA, 1986.
[8]
J. Katz, “Drugs as reinforcers: pharmacological and behavioral factorsin,” in The Neuropharmacological Basis of Reward, J. Leibman and S. Cooper, Eds., pp. 164–213, Clarendon Press, Oxford, UK, 1989.
[9]
C. Iglauer and J. H. Woods, “Concurrent performances: reinforcement by different doses of intravenous cocaine in rhesus monkeys,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 179–196, 1974.
[10]
C. E. Johanson and C. R. Schuster, “A choice procedure for drug reinforcers: cocaine and methylphenidate in the rhesus monkey,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 193, no. 2, pp. 676–688, 1975.
[11]
N. K. Mello and S. S. Negus, “Preclinical evaluation of pharmacotherapies for treatment of cocaine and opioid abuse using drug self-administration procedures,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 14, no. 6, pp. 375–424, 1996.
[12]
G. Zernig, G. Wakonigg, E. Madlung et al., “Do vertical shifts in dose-response rate-relationships in operant conditioning procedures indicate ‘sensitization’ to ‘drug wanting’?” Psychopharmacology, vol. 171, no. 3, pp. 349–363, 2004.
[13]
R. Meisch and G. Lemaire, “Drug self-administration,” in Methods In Behavioral Pharmacology, F. Van Haaren, Ed., pp. 257–300, Elsevier, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993.
[14]
G. Deneau, T. Yanagita, and M. H. Seevers, “Self-administration of psychoactive substances by the monkey-a measure of psychological dependence,” Psychopharmacologia, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 30–48, 1969.
[15]
R. T. Kelleher and S. R. Goldberg, “General introduction: control of drug taking behavior by schedules of reinforcement,” Pharmacological Reviews, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 291–299, 1975.
[16]
R. Pickens and T. Thompson, “Cocaine-reinforced behavior in rats: effects of reinforcement magnitude and fixed-ratio size.,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 161, no. 1, pp. 122–129, 1968.
[17]
M. L. Banks, B. E. Blough, and S. S. Negus, “Effects of monoamine releasers with varying selectivity for releasing dopamine/norepinephrine versus serotonin on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 22, no. 8, pp. 824–836, 2011.
[18]
S. S. Negus and N. K. Mello, “Effects of chronic methadone treatment on cocaine- and food-maintained responding under second-order, progressive-ratio and concurrent-choice schedules in rhesus monkeys,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 74, no. 3, pp. 297–309, 2004.
[19]
S. S. Negus and M. L. Banks, “Making the right choice: lessons from drug discrimination for research on drug reinforcement and drug self-administration,” in Drug DiscrimInation: Applications To MedicInal Chemistry and Drug Studies, R. A. Glennon and R. Young, Eds., pp. 361–388, John Wiley and Sons, Holboken, NJ, USA, 2011.
[20]
S. D. S. Spragg, “Morphine addiction in chimpanzees,” in Comparative Psychology Monographs, pp. 1–132, The John Hopkins Press, Baltimore, Md, USA, 1940.
[21]
J. R. Nichols and W. M. Davis, “Drug addiction. II. Variation of addiction,” Journal of the American Pharmacists Association, vol. 48, pp. 259–262, 1959.
[22]
G. M. Heyman, “Ethanol regulated preference in rats,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 112, no. 2-3, pp. 259–269, 1993.
[23]
S. D. Comer, J. B. Ashworth, R. W. Foltin, C. E. Johanson, J. P. Zacny, and S. L. Walsh, “The role of human drug self-administration procedures in the development of medications,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 96, no. 1-2, pp. 1–15, 2008.
[24]
M. Haney and R. Spealman, “Controversies in translational research: drug self-administration,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 199, no. 3, pp. 403–419, 2008.
[25]
G. H. Heyman, Addiction: A Disorder of Choice, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2009.
[26]
R. Hernstein and D. Prelec, “A theory of addiction,” in Choice Over Time, G. Loewenstein and J. Elster, Eds., pp. 331–360, Russell Sage Press, New York, NY, USA, 1992.
[27]
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, vol. 992, American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, Fourth edition, 2000.
[28]
America Psychiatric Association, http://www.dsm5.org/proposedrevision/Pages/SubstanceUseandAddictiveDisorders.aspx, 2012.
[29]
J. D. Findley, W. W. Robinson, and L. Peregrino, “Addiction to secobarbital and chlordiazepoxide in the rhesus monkey by means of a self-infusion preference procedure,” Psychopharmacologia, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 93–114, 1972.
[30]
C. Iglauer, M. E. Llewellyn, and J. H. Woods, “Concurrent schedules of cocaine injection in rhesus monkeys: dose variations under independent and non independent variable interval procedures,” Pharmacological Reviews, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 367–383, 1975.
[31]
C. E. Johanson, “Pharmacological and environmental variables affecting drug preference in rhesus monkeys,” Pharmacological Reviews, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 343–355, 1975.
[32]
K. G. Anderson and W. L. Woolverton, “Effects of dose and infusion delay on cocaine self-administration choice in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 167, no. 4, pp. 424–430, 2003.
[33]
K. G. Anderson and W. L. Woolverton, “Dose and schedule determinants of cocaine choice under concurrent variable-interval schedules in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 176, no. 3-4, pp. 274–280, 2004.
[34]
M. E. Llewellyn, C. Iglauer, and J. H. Woods, “Relative reinforcer magnitude under a nonindependent concurrent schedule of cocaine reinforcement in rhesus monkeys,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 25, no. 1, pp. 81–91, 1976.
[35]
C. W. Schindler, L. V. Panlilio, and E. B. Thorndike, “Effect of rate of delivery of intravenous cocaine on self-administration in rats,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 93, no. 4, pp. 375–381, 2009.
[36]
R. E. Sorge and P. B. S. Clarke, “Rats self-administer intravenous nicotine delivered in a novel smoking-relevant procedure: effects of dopamine antagonists,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 330, no. 2, pp. 633–640, 2009.
[37]
W. L. Woolverton and K. G. Anderson, “Effects of delay to reinforcement on the choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 186, no. 1, pp. 99–106, 2006.
[38]
W. L. Woolverton and J. K. Rowlett, “Choice maintained by cocaine or food in monkeys: effects of varying probability of reinforcement,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 138, no. 1, pp. 102–106, 1998.
[39]
W. L. Woolverton, “Intravenous self-administration of cocaine under concurrent VI schedules of reinforcement,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 127, no. 3, pp. 195–203, 1996.
[40]
W. L. Woolverton and K. Alling, “Choice under concurrent VI schedules: comparison of behavior maintained by cocaine or food,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 141, no. 1, pp. 47–56, 1999.
[41]
K. G. Anderson and W. L. Woolverton, “Concurrent variable-interval drug self-administration and the generalized matching law: a drug-class comparison,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 11, no. 5, pp. 413–420, 2000.
[42]
M. N. Koffarnus and J. H. Woods, “Quantification of drug choice with the generalized matching law in rhesus monkeys,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 89, no. 2, pp. 209–224, 2008.
[43]
S. R. Hursh, “Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 34, no. 2, pp. 219–238, 1980.
[44]
W. K. Bickel, R. J. DeGrandpre, and S. T. Higgins, “The behavioral economics of concurrent drug reinforcers: a review and reanalysis of drug self administration research,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 118, no. 3, pp. 250–259, 1995.
[45]
S. R. Hursh and R. Bauman, “The behavioral analysis of demand,” in Advances In Behavioral Economics, L. Green and J. Kagel, Eds., pp. 117–165, Ablex Publishing, Norwood, Mass, USA, 1987.
[46]
C. E. Johanson and T. Aigner, “Comparison of the reinforcing properties of cocaine and procaine in rhesus monkeys,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 49–53, 1981.
[47]
W. L. Woolverton and C. E. Johanson, “Preference in rhesus monkeys given a choice between cocaine and d,l-cathinone.,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 35–43, 1984.
[48]
A. M. Manzardo, J. A. Del Rio, L. Stein, and J. D. Belluzzi, “Rats choose cocaine over dopamine agonists in a two-lever self-administration preference test,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 70, no. 2-3, pp. 257–265, 2001.
[49]
A. M. Manzardo, L. Stein, and J. D. Belluzzi, “Rats prefer cocaine over nicotine in a two-lever self-administration choice test,” Brain Research, vol. 924, no. 1, pp. 10–19, 2002.
[50]
J. A. Lile, D. Morgan, A. M. Birmingham et al., “The reinforcing efficacy of the dopamine reuptake inhibitor 2β-propanoyl-3β-(4-tolyl)-tropane (PTT) as measured by a progressive-ratio schedule and a choice procedure in rhesus monkeys,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 303, no. 2, pp. 640–648, 2002.
[51]
T. Wade-Galuska, G. Winger, and J. H. Woods, “A behavioral economic analysis of cocaine and remifentanil self-administration in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 194, no. 4, pp. 563–572, 2007.
[52]
D. Caprioli, M. Celentano, A. Dubla, F. Lucantonio, P. Nencini, and A. Badiani, “Ambience and drug choice: cocaine- and heroin-taking as a function of environmental context in humans and rats,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 65, no. 10, pp. 893–899, 2009.
[53]
T. G. Aigner and R. L. Balster, “Choice behavior in rhesus monkeys: cocaine versus food,” Science, vol. 201, no. 4355, pp. 534–535, 1978.
[54]
C. A. Paronis, M. Gasior, and J. Bergman, “Effects of cocaine under concurrent fixed ratio schedules of food and IV drug availability: a novel choice procedure in monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 163, no. 3-4, pp. 283–291, 2002.
[55]
S. S. Negus, “Rapid assessment of choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys: effects of environmental manipulations and treatment with d-amphetamine and flupenthixol,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 28, no. 5, pp. 919–931, 2003.
[56]
P. W. Czoty and M. A. Nader, “Individual differences in the effects of environmental stimuli on cocaine choice in socially housed male cynomolgus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 224, no. 1, pp. 69–79, 2012.
[57]
M. A. Nader and W. L. Woolverton, “Effects of increasing the magnitude of an alternative reinforcer on drug choice in a discrete-trials choice procedure,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 105, no. 2, pp. 169–174, 1991.
[58]
M. A. Nader and W. L. Woolverton, “Effects of increasing response requirement on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 295–300, 1992.
[59]
M. A. Nader and W. L. Woolverton, “Choice between cocaine and food by rhesus monkeys: effects of conditions of food availability,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 3, no. 6, pp. 635–638, 1992.
[60]
T. F. Elsmore, G. V. Fletcher, D. G. Conrad, and F. J. Sodetz, “Reduction of heroin intake in baboons by an economic constraint,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 729–731, 1980.
[61]
M. Thomsen, A. Fink-Jensen, D. P. D. Woldbye et al., “Effects of acute and chronic aripiprazole treatment on choice between cocaine self-administration and food under a concurrent schedule of reinforcement in rats,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 201, no. 1, pp. 43–53, 2008.
[62]
M. E. Carroll, S. T. Lac, and S. L. Nygaard, “A concurrently available nondrug reinforcer prevents the acquisition or decreases the maintenance of cocaine-reinforced behavior,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 97, no. 1, pp. 23–29, 1989.
[63]
S. I. Dworkin, S. Mirkis, and J. E. Smith, “Reinforcer interactions under concurrent schedules of food, water, and intravenous cocaine,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 327–338, 1990.
[64]
A. Ping and P. J. Kruzich, “Concurrent access to sucrose pellets decreases methamphetamine-seeking behavior in Lewis rats,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 90, no. 3, pp. 492–496, 2008.
[65]
M. Lenoir, F. Serre, L. Cantin, and S. H. Ahmed, “Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward,” PloS one, vol. 2, no. 1, p. e698, 2007.
[66]
R. M. Wurster, R. R. Griffiths, J. D. Findley, and J. V. Brady, “Reduction of heroin self-administration in baboons by manipulation of behavioral and pharmacological conditions,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 7, no. 6, pp. 519–528, 1977.
[67]
S. S. Negus, “Interactions between the reinforcing effects of cocaine and heroin in a drug-vs-food choice procedure in rhesus monkeys: a dose-addition analysis,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 180, no. 1, pp. 115–124, 2005.
[68]
S. J. Ward, D. Morgan, and D. C. S. Roberts, “Comparison of the reinforcing effects of cocaine and cocaine/heroin combinations under progressive ratio and choice schedules in rats,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 286–295, 2005.
[69]
K. B. Freeman and W. L. Woolverton, “Self-administration of cocaine and remifentanil by monkeys: choice between single drugs and mixtures,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 215, no. 2, pp. 281–290, 2011.
[70]
G. W. Stevenson, J. E. Folk, K. C. Rice, and S. S. Negus, “Interactions between δ and μ opioid agonists in assays of schedule-controlled responding, thermal nociception, drug self-administration, and drug versus food choice in rhesus monkeys: studies with SNC80 [(+)-4-[(αR)-α-((2S,5R)-4-allyl-2,5-dimethyl-1-piperazinyl) -3-methoxybenzyl]-N,N-diethylbenzamide] and heroin,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 314, no. 1, pp. 221–231, 2005.
[71]
C. E. Johanson, “The effects of electric shock on responding maintained by cocaine injections in a choice procedure in the rhesus monkey,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 53, no. 3, pp. 277–282, 1977.
[72]
S. S. Negus, “Effects of punishment on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 181, no. 2, pp. 244–252, 2005.
[73]
W. L. Woolverton, K. B. Freeman, J. Myerson, and L. Green, “Suppression of cocaine self-administration in monkeys: effects of delayed punishment,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 220, no. 3, pp. 509–517, 2012.
[74]
R. R. Griffiths, R. M. Wurster, and J. V. Brady, “Discrete trial choice procedure: effects of naloxone and methadone on choice between food and heroin,” Pharmacological Reviews, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 357–365, 1975.
[75]
R. R. Griffiths, R. M. Wurster, and J. V. Brady, “Choice between food and heroin: effects of morphine, naloxone, and secobarbital.,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 335–351, 1981.
[76]
S. S. Negus, “Choice between heroin and food in nondependent and heroin-dependent rhesus monkeys: effects of naloxone, buprenorphine, and methadone,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 317, no. 2, pp. 711–723, 2006.
[77]
S. S. Negus, “Opioid antagonist effects in animal models related to opioid abuse: drug discrimination and drug self-administration,” in Opiate RecepTors and Antagonists: From Bench To Clinic, R. Dean, E. J. Bilsky, and S. S. Negus, Eds., pp. 201–226, Humana Press, New York, NY, USA, 2009.
[78]
T. Wade-Galuska, C. M. Galuska, and G. Winger, “Effects of daily morphine administration and deprivation on choice and demand for remifentanil and cocaine in rhesus monkeys,” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, vol. 95, no. 1, pp. 75–89, 2011.
[79]
S. S. Negus and K. C. Rice, “Mechanisms of withdrawal-associated increases in heroin self-administration: pharmacologic modulation of heroin vs food choice in heroin-dependent rhesus monkeys,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 899–911, 2009.
[80]
M. L. Banks and S. S. Negus, “Effects of extended cocaine access and cocaine withdrawal on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 35, no. 2, pp. 493–504, 2010.
[81]
M. Thomsen, A. Barrett, S. S. Negus, and S. B. Caine, “Cocaine-food choice rats: environmental manipulations and effects of amphetamine,” Behavioural Pharmacology. In press.
[82]
E. Augier, C. Vouillac, and S. H. Ahmed, “Diazepam promotes choice of abstinence in cocaine self-administering rats,” Addiction Biology, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 378–391, 2012.
[83]
P. W. Czoty, C. McCabe, and M. A. Nader, “Effects of the 5-HT1A agonist (±)-8-hydroxy-2-(di-n- propylamino)tetralin (8-OH-DPAT) on cocaine choice in cynomolgus monkeys,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 187–191, 2005.
[84]
S. S. Negus, “Effects of the kappa opioid agonist U50,488 and the kappa opioid antagonist nor-binaltorphimine on choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 176, no. 2, pp. 204–213, 2004.
[85]
W. L. Woolverton and R. L. Balster, “Effects of antipsychotic compounds in rhesus monkeys given a choice between cocaine and food,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 69–78, 1981.
[86]
W. L. Woolverton and R. L. Balster, “The effects of lithium on choice between cocaine and food in the rhesus monkey,” Communications in Psychopharmacology, vol. 3, no. 5, pp. 309–318, 1979.
[87]
F. J. Vocci, “Commentary: can replacement therapy work in the treatment of cocaine dependence? and what are we replacing anyway?” Addiction, vol. 102, no. 12, pp. 1888–1889, 2007.
[88]
M. K. Greenwald, L. H. Lundahl, and C. L. Steinmiller, “Sustained release d-amphetamine reduces cocaine but not speedball-seeking in buprenorphine-maintained volunteers: a test of dual-agonist pharmacotherapy for cocaine/heroin polydrug abusers,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 35, no. 13, pp. 2624–2637, 2010.
[89]
C. R. Rush, W. W. Stoops, R. J. Sevak, and L. R. Hays, “Cocaine choice in humans during D-amphetamine maintenance,” Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 152–159, 2010.
[90]
S. L. Walsh, B. Geter-Douglas, E. C. Strain, and G. E. Bigelow, “Enadoline and butorphanol: evaluation of κ-agonists on cocaine pharmacodynamics and cocaine self-administration in humans,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 299, no. 1, pp. 147–158, 2001.
[91]
P. W. Czoty, C. McCabe, and M. A. Nader, “Assessment of the relative reinforcing strength of cocaine in socially housed monkeys using a choice procedure,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 312, no. 1, pp. 96–102, 2005.
[92]
M. L. Banks, J. E. Sprague, P. W. Czoty, and M. A. Nader, “Effects of ambient temperature on the relative reinforcing strength of MDMA using a choice procedure in monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 196, no. 1, pp. 63–70, 2008.
[93]
J. Bergman, “Medications for stimulant abuse: agonist-based strategies and preclinical evaluation of the mixed-action D2 partial agonist aripiprazole (Abilify?),” Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, vol. 16, no. 6, pp. 475–483, 2008.
[94]
M. L. Banks, P. W. Czoty, J. E. Sprague, and M. A. Nader, “Influence of thyroid hormones on 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine-induced thermogenesis and reinforcing strength in monkeys,” Behavioural Pharmacology, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 167–170, 2008.
[95]
R. L. Corwin and C. R. Schuster, “Anorectic specificity as measured in a choice paradigm in rhesus monkeys,” Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 131–141, 1993.
[96]
L. A. Marsch and W. K. Bickel, “Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes,” Addiction, vol. 96, no. 1, pp. 73–86, 2001.
[97]
J. Monterosso, P. Piray, and S. Luo, “Neuroeconomics and the study of addiction,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 107–112, 2012.
[98]
C. A. Winstanley, “The utility of rat models of impulsivity in developing pharmacotherapies for impulse control disorders,” British Journal of Pharmacology, vol. 164, no. 4, pp. 1301–1321, 2011.
[99]
W. L. Woolverton, J. A. English, and M. R. Weed, “Choice between cocaine and food in a discrete-trials procedure in monkeys: a unit price analysis,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 133, no. 3, pp. 269–274, 1997.
[100]
W. L. Woolverton and J. A. English, “Further analysis of choice between cocaine and food using the unit price model of behavioral economics,” Drug and Alcohol Dependence, vol. 49, no. 1, pp. 71–78, 1997.
[101]
K. G. Anderson, A. J. Velkey, and W. L. Woolverton, “The generalized matching law as a predictor of choice between cocaine and food in rhesus monkeys,” Psychopharmacology, vol. 163, no. 3-4, pp. 319–326, 2002.
[102]
M. Gasior, J. Bergman, M. J. Kallman, and C. A. Paronis, “Evaluation of the reinforcing effects of monoamine reuptake inhibitors under a concurrent schedule of food and i.v. drug delivery in rhesus monkeys,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 758–764, 2005.
[103]
M. Gasior, C. A. Paronis, and J. Bergman, “Modification by dopaminergic drugs of choice behavior under concurrent schedules of intravenous saline and food delivery in monkeys,” Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, vol. 308, no. 1, pp. 249–259, 2004.