Background Converging evidence indicates that action observation and action-related sounds activate cross-modally the human motor system. Since olfaction, the most ancestral sense, may have behavioural consequences on human activities, we causally investigated by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) whether food odour could additionally facilitate the human motor system during the observation of grasping objects with alimentary valence, and the degree of specificity of these effects. Methodology/Principal Findings In a repeated-measure block design, carried out on 24 healthy individuals participating to three different experiments, we show that sniffing alimentary odorants immediately increases the motor potentials evoked in hand muscles by TMS of the motor cortex. This effect was odorant-specific and was absent when subjects were presented with odorants including a potentially noxious trigeminal component. The smell-induced corticospinal facilitation of hand muscles during observation of grasping was an additive effect which superimposed to that induced by the mere observation of grasping actions for food or non-food objects. The odour-induced motor facilitation took place only in case of congruence between the sniffed odour and the observed grasped food, and specifically involved the muscle acting as prime mover for hand/fingers shaping in the observed action. Conclusions/Significance Complex olfactory cross-modal effects on the human corticospinal system are physiologically demonstrable. They are odorant-specific and, depending on the experimental context, muscle- and action-specific as well. This finding implies potential new diagnostic and rehabilitative applications.
References
[1]
Fontanini A, Bower JM (2006) Slow-waves in the olfactory system: an olfactory perspective on cortical rhythms. Trends Neurosci. 29: 429–437.
[2]
Spence C, Kettenmann B, Kobal G, McGlone FP (2001) Shared attentional resources for processing visual and chemosensory information. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 54: 775–783.
[3]
Herz RS, Eliassen J, Beland S, Souza T (2004) Neuroimaging evidence for the emotional potency of odor-evoked memory. Neuropsychologia 42: 371–8.
[4]
Herz RS (1998) Are odors the best cues to memory? A cross-modal comparison of associative memory stimuli. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 855: 670–674.
Bensafi M, Porter J, Pouliot S, Mainland J, Johnson B, et al. (2003) Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nat. Neurosci. 11: 1142–1144.
[7]
Porter J, Craven B, Khan RM, Chang SJ, Kang I, et al. (2006) Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 10: 27–29.
[8]
Castiello U, Zucco GM, Parma V, Ansuini C, Tirindelli R (2006) Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and vision when grasping. Chem. Senses 31: 665–71.
[9]
Hallett M (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature 406: 147–150.
[10]
Rossini PM, Rossi S (2007) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: diagnostic, therapeutic and research potential. Neurology 68: 484–488.
[11]
Lemon RN, Johansson RS, Westling G (1995) Corticospinal control during reach, grasp, and precision lift in man. J. Neurosci. 15: 6145–615.
[12]
Abbruzzese G, Trompetto C, Schieppati M (1996) The excitability of the human motor cortex increases during execution and mental imagination of sequential but not repetitive finger movements. Exp. Brain Res. 111: 465–72.
[13]
Rossi S, Pasqualetti P, Tecchio F, Pauri F, Rossini PM (1998) Corticospinal excitability modulation during mental simulation of wrist movements in human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 243: 147–151.
[14]
Rossini PM, Rossi S, Pasqualetti P, Tecchio F (1999) Corticospinal excitability modulation to hand muscles during movement imagery. Cereb. Cortex 9: 161–167.
[15]
Fadiga L, Buccino G, Craighero L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, et al. (1999) Corticospinal excitability is specifically modulated by motor imagery: a magnetic stimulation study. Neuropsychologia 37: 147–58.
[16]
Clark S, Tremblay F, Ste-Marie D (2004) Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions. Neuropsychologia 42: 105–112.
[17]
Rossi S, Rossini PM (2004) TMS in cognitive plasticity and the potential for rehabilitation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8: 273–279.
[18]
Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Pavesi G, Rizzolatti G (1995) Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73: 2608–2611.
[19]
Gangitano M, Mottaghy FM, Pascual-Leone A (2001) Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. Neuroreport 12: 1489–1492.
[20]
Gangitano M, Mottaghy FM, Pascual-Leone A (2004) Modulation of premotor mirror neuron activity during observation of unpredictable grasping movements. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20: 2193–2202.
[21]
Fadiga L, Craighero L, Olivier E (2005) Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others' action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15: 213–8.
[22]
Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3: 131–141.
[23]
Kohler E, Keysers C, Umilta' MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, et al. (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297: 846–848.
[24]
Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27: 169–192.
[25]
Tettamanti M, Buccino G, Saccuman MC, Gallese V, Danna M, et al. (2005) Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17: 273–81.
[26]
Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C (2006) Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr. Biol. 16: 1824–1829.
[27]
Cheng Y, Metzoff AN, Decety J (2006) Motivation modulates the activity of the human mirror-neuron system. Cer. Cortex epub ahead of print.
[28]
Aziz-Zadeh L, Iacoboni M, Zaidel E, Wilson S, Mazziotta J (2004) Left hemisphere motor facilitation in response to manual action sounds. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19: 2609–2612.
[29]
Royet JP, Zald D, Versace R, Costes N, Lavenne F, et al. (2000) Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant olfactory, visual, and auditory stimuli: a positron emission tomography study. J. Neurosci. 20: 7752–9.
[30]
Rothman KJEpidemiology. An introduction. Oxford University Press
[31]
Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M, Evans AC, Meyer E (1992) Functional localisation of human olfactory cortex. Nature 360: 339–341.
[32]
Savic I (2002) Imaging of brain activation by odorants in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12: 455–61.
[33]
Sobel N, Prabhakaran V, Zhao Z, Desmond JE, Glover GH, et al. (2000) Time course of odorant-induced activation in the human primary olfactory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 83: 537–51.
[34]
Savic I, Gulyas B, Berglund H (2002) Odorant differentiated pattern of cerebral activation: comparison of acetone and vanillin. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17: 17–27.
[35]
Le Pera D, Graven-Nielsen T, Valeriani M, Oliviero A, Di Lazzaro V, et al. (2001) Inhibition of motor system excitability at cortical and spinal level by tonic muscle pain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112: 1633–1641.
[36]
Svensson P, Miles TS, McKay D, Ridding MC (2003) Suppression of motor evoked potentials in a hand muscle following prolonged painful stimulation. Eur. J. Pain 7: 55–62.
[37]
Avenanti A, Bueti D, Galati G, Aglioti SM (2005) Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nat. Neurosci. 8: 955–960.
Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L (1998) Grasping objects and grasping action meanings: the dual role of monkey rostroventral premotor cortex (area F5). Novartis Found. Symp. 218: 81–95.
[40]
Maeda F, Kleiner-Fisman G, Pasacual-Leone A (2002) Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer's orientation. J. Neurophysiol. 87: 1329–35.
[41]
Ferrari PF, Maiolini C, Addessi E, Fogassi I, Visalberghi E (2005) The observation and hearing of eating actions activates motor programs related to eating in macaque monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 161: 95–101.
[42]
Picton TW (1992) The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 9: 456–479.
[43]
Iacoboni M, Dapretto M (2006) The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunctions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7: 942–951.
[44]
Ponsen MM, Stoffers D, Booij J, van Eck-Smit BL, Wolters ECh, et al. (2004) Idiopathic hyposmia as a preclinical sign of Parkinson's disease. Ann. Neurol. 56: 173–81.
[45]
Rossini PM, et al. (1994) Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91: 79–92.
[46]
Rossi S, Pasqualetti P, Tecchio F, Sabato A, Rossini PM (1998) Modulation of corticospinal output to human hand muscles following deprivation of sensory feedback. Neuroimage 8: 163–175.
Filippi MM, Oliveri M, Vernieri F, Pasqualetti P, Rossini PM (2000) Are autonomic signals influencing cortico-spinal motor excitability? A study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res. 881: 159–64.
[49]
Nielsen JF (1996) Logarithmic distribution of amplitudes of compound muscle action potentials evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 13: 423–34.