全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

Mirroring Intentional Forgetting in a Shared-Goal Learning Situation

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029992

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Background Intentional forgetting refers to the surprising phenomenon that we can forget previously successfully encoded memories if we are instructed to do so. Here, we show that participants cannot only intentionally forget episodic memories but they can also mirror the “forgetting performance” of an observed model. Methodology/Principal Findings In four experiments a participant observed a model who took part in a memory experiment. In Experiment 1 and 2 observers saw a movie about the experiment, whereas in Experiment 3 and 4 the observers and the models took part together in a real laboratory experiment. The observed memory experiment was a directed forgetting experiment where the models learned two lists of items and were instructed either to forget or to remember the first list. In Experiment 1 and 3 observers were instructed to simply observe the experiment (“simple observation” instruction). In Experiment 2 and 4, observers received instructions aimed to induce the same learning goal for the observers and the models (“observation with goal-sharing” instruction). A directed forgetting effect (the reliably lower recall of to-be-forgotten items) emerged only when models received the “observation with goal-sharing” instruction (P<.001 in Experiment 2, and P<.05 in Experiment 4), and it was absent when observers received the “simple observation” instruction (P>.1 in Experiment 1 and 3). Conclusion If people observe another person with the same intention to learn, and see that this person is instructed to forget previously studied information, then they will produce the same intentional forgetting effect as the person they observed. This seems to be a an important aspect of human learning: if we can understand the goal of an observed person and this is in line with our behavioural goals then our learning performance will mirror the learning performance of the model.

References

[1]  Bjork RA (1989) Retrieval inhibition as an adaptive mechanism in human memory. In: Roediger HL, Craik FIM, editors. Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving. Hilsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 309–330.
[2]  MacLeod C (1998) Directed forgetting. In: Golding JM, MacLeod , CM , editors. Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–58.
[3]  Conway MA, Harries K, Noyes J, Racsmány M, Frankish CR (2000) The disruption and dissolution of directed forgetting: Inhibitory control of memory. J Mem Lang 43: 409–430.
[4]  Hasher L, Zacks R (1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In: Bower GH, editor. The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (vol 22). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. pp. 193–225.
[5]  Ludowig E, M?ller J, Bien C, Münte TF, Elger CE, et al. (2010) Active suppression in the mediotemporal lobe during directed forgetting. Neurobiol Learn Mem 93: 352–361.
[6]  B?uml KH, Past?tter B, Hanslmayr S (2010) Binding and inhibition in episodic memory-Cognitive, emotional, and neural processes. Neurosci Biobehav R 34: 1047–1054.
[7]  Van Hooff JC, Whitaker TA, Ford RM (2009) Directed forgetting in direct and indirect tests of memory: Seeking evidence of retrieval inhibition using electrophysiological measures. Brain Cognition 71: 153–164.
[8]  Banich MT, Mackiewicz KL, Depue BE, Whitmer AJ, Miller GA, et al. (2009) Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: A neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav R 33: 613–630.
[9]  Reber PJ, Siwiec RM, Gitleman DR, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM, et al. (2002) Neural correlates of successful encoding identified using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 22: 9541–9548.
[10]  Ullsperger M, Mecklinger A, Müller U (2000) An electrophysiological test of directed forgetting: The role of retrieval inhibition. J Cognitive Neurosci 12: 924–940.
[11]  Conway MA, Fthenaki A (2003) Disruption of inhibitory control of memory following lesions to the frontal and temporal lobes. Cortex 39: 667–686.
[12]  Anderson MC, Green C (2001) Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control. Nature 410: 366–369.
[13]  Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kuhl B, Cooper J, Robertson E, et al. (2004) Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 303: 232–235.
[14]  Racsmány M, Conway MA (2006) Episodic inhibition. J Exp Psychol Learn 32: 44–57.
[15]  Mecklinger A, Parra M, Waldhauser GT (2009) ERP correlates of intentional forgetting. Brain Res 1255: 132–147.
[16]  Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cog Sci 8: 170–177.
[17]  B?uml KH, S , Past?tter B, Klimesch W (2008) Oscillatory correlates of intentional updating in episodic memory. NeuroImage 41: 596–604.
[18]  Keefe RSE (2000) Working memory dysfunction and its relevance in schizophrenia. In: Sharma , T , Harvey , PD , editors. Cognitive Functioning in Schizophrenia. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 16–49.
[19]  Racsmány M, Conway MA, Garab EA, Cimmer Cs, Janka Z, et al. (2008) Disrupted memory inhibition in schizophrenia. Schiz Res 101: 218–224.
[20]  Sahakyan L, Kelley CM (2002) A contextual change account of directed forgetting effect. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 28: 1064–1072.
[21]  Sahakyan L, Delaney PF (2003) Can encoding differences explain the benefits of directed forgetting in the list method paradigm? J Mem Lang 48: 195–206.
[22]  Sahakyan L, Delaney PF (2005) Directed Forgetting in Incidental Learning and Recognition Testing: Support for a Two-Factor Account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31: 789–801.
[23]  Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Res 3: 131–141.
[24]  Buccino G, Binkofski F, Riggio L (2004) The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain Lang 89: 370–376.
[25]  Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8: 396–403.
[26]  Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27: 169–192.
[27]  Welsh TN, Elliott D, Anson JG, Dhillon V, Weeks DJ, et al. (2005) Does Joe influence Fred's action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems. Neurosci Lett 385: 99–104.
[28]  Frischen A, Loach D, Tipper SP (2009) Seeing the world through another person's eyes: Simulating selective attention via action observation. Cognition 111: 212–218.
[29]  Conway MA (2005) Memory and the self. J Mem Lang 53: 594–628.
[30]  Bjork EL, Bjork RA (1996) Continuing influences of to-be-forgotten information. Conscious Cogn 5: 176–196.
[31]  Csibra G (2007) Action mirroring and action understanding: An alternative account. In: Haggard , P , Rosetti , Y , Kawato , M , editors. Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Attention and Performance. XXII. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 435–459.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133