[1] | Lyons DE, Young AG, Keil FC (2007) The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 104: 19751–19756.
|
[2] | Nielsen M (2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology 42: 555–565.
|
[3] | Kenward B, Karlsson M, Persson J (2011) Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278: 1239–1246.
|
[4] | Horner V, Whiten A (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition 8: 164–181.
|
[5] | Csibra G, Gergely G (2006) Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In: Johnson MH, Munakata YM, editors. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development Attention and Performance, XXI. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 249–274.
|
[6] | Gergely G, Csibra G (2005) The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies 6: 463–481.
|
[7] | Schmidt M, Rakoczy H, Tomasello M (2011) Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science 14: 530–539.
|
[8] | McGuigan N, Graham M (2010) Cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 7: 561–577.
|
[9] | Flynn E (2008) Investigating children as cultural magnets: do young children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London B 363: 3541–3551.
|
[10] | Flynn E, Whiten A (2010) Studying children's social learning exerimentally “in the wild”. Learning & Behavior 38: 284–296.
|
[11] | Nielsen M, Tomaselli K (2010) Over-imitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science 21: 729–736.
|
[12] | McGuigan N, Makinson J, Whiten A (2011) From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology 102: 1–18.
|
[13] | Whiten A (2005) The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature 437: 52–55.
|
[14] | Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
|
[15] | Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
|
[16] | Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
|
[17] | Rakoczy H (2007) Play, games, and the development of collective intentionality. In: Kalish C, Sabbagh M, editors. Conventionality in cognitive development: How children acquire representations in language, thought and action New directions in child and adolescent development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. pp. 53–67.
|
[18] | Searle JR (1995) The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.
|
[19] | Leslie A (1987) Pretence and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review 94: 412–426.
|
[20] | Perner J (1991) Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
|
[21] | Harris PL, Kavanaugh RD (1993) Young children's understanding of pretence. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58:
|
[22] | Lillard AS (1993) Young children's conceptualisation of pretence: Action or mental representational state? Child Development 64: 372–386.
|
[23] | Rakoczy H (2008) Pretence as individual and collective intentionality. Mind & Language 23: 499–517.
|
[24] | Nielsen M (in press) Imitation, pretend play and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology.
|
[25] | Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010) The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33: 61–135.
|
[26] | Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A (2010) Most people are not WEIRD. Nature 466: 29.
|
[27] | Haight WL, Wang X, Han-tih H, Williams K, Mintz J (1999) Universal, developmental, and variable aspects of young children's play: A cross-cultural comparison of pretending at home. Child Development 70: 1477–1488.
|
[28] | Smith PK (2010) Children and play. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
|
[29] | Nielsen M, Hudry K (2010) Over-imitation in children with Autism and Down syndrome. Australian Journal of Psychology 62: 67–74.
|
[30] | Whiten A, McGuigan N, Marshall-Pescini S, Hopper LM (2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London B 364: 2417–2428.
|
[31] | Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London B 364: 2405–2415.
|
[32] | Flynn E, Whiten A (2008) Cultural transmission of tool use in young children: A diffusion chain study. Social Development 17: 699–718.
|
[33] | Csibra G, Gergely G (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science 13: 148–153.
|
[34] | Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvia's recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Levenson S, Enfield N, editors. Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction. Oxford: Berg Publishers. pp. 229–255.
|
[35] | Gergely G, Egyed K, Kiraly I (2007) On pedagogy. Developmental Science 10: 139–146.
|
[36] | Zmyj N, Daum MM, Prinz W, Nielsen M, Aschersleben G (in press) Fourteen-month-olds' imitation of differently aged models. Infant and Child Development.
|
[37] | McGuigan N (in press) The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions. Journal of Comparative Psychology.
|
[38] | Vygotsky LS (1978) Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
|
[39] | Over H, Carpenter M (in press) Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology.
|
[40] | Nielsen M, Blank C (2011) Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology 47: 1050–1053.
|
[41] | Kenward B (in press) Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
|
[42] | Lyons DE, Damrosch DH, Lin JK, Macris DM, Keil FC (2011) The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1158–1167.
|
[43] | Nagell K, Olguin RS, Tomasello M (1993) Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 107: 174–186.
|
[44] | Carpenter M, Nielsen M (2008) Tools, TV and trust: Introduction to the special issue on imitation in typically developing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 101: 225–227.
|
[45] | Rakoczy H, Tomasello M, Striano T (2005) How children turn objects into symbols: A cultural learning account. In: Namy L, editor. Symbol use and symbol representation. New York: Erlbaum. pp. 67–97.
|
[46] | Bretherton I, O'Connell B, Shore C, Bates E (1984) The effect of contextual variation on symbolic play: Development from 20 to 28 months. In: Bretherton I, editor. Symbolic play: The development of social understanding. New York: Academic Press. pp. 271–298.
|
[47] | McCune-Nicolich L (1981) Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development 52: 785–797.
|