Are we born amoral or do we come into this world with a rudimentary moral compass? Hamlin and colleagues argue that at least one component of our moral system, the ability to evaluate other individuals as good or bad, is present from an early age. In their study, 6- and 10-month-old infants watched two social interactions - in one, infants observed the helper assist the climber achieve the goal of ascending a hill, while in the other, infants observed the hinderer prevent the climber from ascending the hill. When given a choice, the vast majority of infants picked the helper over the hinderer, suggesting that infants evaluated the helper as good and the hinderer as bad. Hamlin and colleagues concluded that the ability to evaluate individuals based on social interaction is innate. Here, we provide evidence that their findings reflect simple associations rather than social evaluations.
References
[1]
Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2007) Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450: 557–559.
[2]
Haith MM (1998) Who put the cog in infant cognition? Is rich interpretation too costly? Infant Behav Dev 21: 167–179.
[3]
Spelke ES (1998) Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge. Infant Behav Dev 21: 181–200.
[4]
Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2010) Three-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Sci 13: 923–929.
[5]
Farroni T, Menon E, Rigato S, Johnson MH (2007) The perception of facial expressions in newborns. Eur J Dev Psychol 4: 2–13.
[6]
LaBarbera JD, Izard CE, Vietze P, Parisi SA (1976) Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. Child Dev 47: 535–538.
[7]
Hamlin JK, Wynn K (2011) Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Dev 26: 30–39.
[8]
Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P, Mahajan N (2011) How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108: 19931–19936.
[9]
Scarf D, Imuta K, Colombo M, Hayne H (2012) Golden rule or valence matching? Methodological problems in Hamlin et al. P Natl Acad Sci USA 109: E1426.
[10]
Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P (2012) Reply to Scarf et al.: Nuanced social evaluation: Association doesn't compute. P Natl Acad Sci USA 109: E1427.
[11]
Lewkowicz DJ (2011) The biological implausibility of the nature-nurture dichotomy and what it means for the study of infancy. Infancy 16: 331–367.
[12]
Prinz J (2009) Against moral nativism. In: Stich and his Critics. Murphy D, Bishop M, editors. Massachusetts: Wiley-Blackwell. 167–189.
[13]
Sterelny K (2010) Moral nativism: A sceptical response. Mind Lang 25: 279–297.
[14]
Perner J, Ruffman T (2005) Infants' insight into the mind: How deep? Science 308: 214–216.
[15]
Ruffman T, Taumoepeau M, Perkins C (2011) Statistical learning as a basis for social understanding in children. Brit J Dev Psychol 30: 87–104.
[16]
Sirois S, Jackson I (2007) Social cognition in infancy: A critical review of research on higher order abilities. Eur J Dev Psychol 4: 46–64.
[17]
Balter M (2012) ‘Killjoys’ challenge claims of clever animals. Science 335: 1036–1037.
[18]
Colombo M, Hayne H (2010) Episodic memory: Comparative and developmental issues. In: Blumberg MS, Freeman JH, Robinson SR, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Behavioral Neuroscience.New York: Oxford University Press. 617–636.
[19]
Le Pelley ME (2012) Metacognitive monkeys or associative animals? Simple reinforcement learning explains uncertainty in nonhuman animals. J Exp Psychol Learn 38: 686–708.
[20]
van der Vaart E, Verbrugge R, Hemelrijk CK (2012) Corvid Re-Caching without ‘Theory of Mind’: A Model. PLoS ONE 7: e32904.
[21]
Darwin C (1982) The Origin of Species. California: Penguin Classics. 480.