全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
PLOS ONE  2012 

The Effect of Motivation on Movement: A Study of Bradykinesia in Parkinson’s Disease

DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047138

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Background Bradykinesia is a cardinal feature of Parkinson’s disease (PD). Despite its disabling impact, the precise cause of this symptom remains elusive. Recent thinking suggests that bradykinesia may be more than simply a manifestation of motor slowness, and may in part reflect a specific deficit in the operation of motivational vigour in the striatum. In this paper we test the hypothesis that movement time in PD can be modulated by the specific nature of the motivational salience of possible action-outcomes. Methodology/Principal Findings We developed a novel movement time paradigm involving winnable rewards and avoidable painful electrical stimuli. The faster the subjects performed an action the more likely they were to win money (in appetitive blocks) or to avoid a painful shock (in aversive blocks). We compared PD patients when OFF dopaminergic medication with controls. Our key finding is that PD patients OFF dopaminergic medication move faster to avoid aversive outcomes (painful electric shocks) than to reap rewarding outcomes (winning money) and, unlike controls, do not speed up in the current trial having failed to win money in the previous one. We also demonstrate that sensitivity to distracting stimuli is valence specific. Conclusions/Significance We suggest this pattern of results can be explained in terms of low dopamine levels in the Parkinsonian state leading to an insensitivity to appetitive outcomes, and thus an inability to modulate movement speed in the face of rewards. By comparison, sensitivity to aversive stimuli is relatively spared. Our findings point to a rarely described property of bradykinesia in PD, namely its selective regulation by everyday outcomes.

References

[1]  Edwards MQ N, Bhatia K (2008) Parkinson’s disease and other movement disorders. Oxford University Press.
[2]  Montgomery EB Jr, Nuessen J (1990) The movement speed/accuracy operator in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 40: 269–272.
[3]  Sheridan MR, Flowers KA (1990) Movement variability and bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 113 (Pt 4): 1149–1161.
[4]  Teasdale N, Phillips J, Stelmach GE (1990) Temporal movement control in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 53: 862–868.
[5]  Hallett M, Khoshbin S (1980) A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain 103: 301–314.
[6]  Sheridan MR, Flowers KA, Hurrell J (1987) Programming and execution of movement in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 110 (Pt 5): 1247–1271.
[7]  Berardelli A, Dick JP, Rothwell JC, Day BL, Marsden CD (1986) Scaling of the size of the first agonist EMG burst during rapid wrist movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 49: 1273–1279.
[8]  Mazzoni P, Hristova A, Krakauer JW (2007) Why don’t we move faster? Parkinson’s disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J Neurosci 27: 7105–7116.
[9]  Salamone JD, Correa M (2002) Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav Brain Res 137: 3–25.
[10]  Niv Y, Daw ND, Joel D, Dayan P (2007) Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191: 507–520.
[11]  Moustafa AA, Cohen MX, Sherman SJ, Frank MJ (2008) A role for dopamine in temporal decision making and reward maximization in parkinsonism. J Neurosci 28: 12294–12304.
[12]  Blin O, Ferrandez AM, Serratrice G (1990) Quantitative analysis of gait in Parkinson patients: increased variability of stride length. J Neurol Sci 98: 91–97.
[13]  Critchley M (1929) Arteriosclerotic Parkinsonism. Brain 52: 23–83.
[14]  Rahman S, Griffin HJ, Quinn NP, Jahanshahi M (2008) The factors that induce or overcome freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease. Behav Neurol 19: 127–136.
[15]  Praamstra P, Stegeman DF, Cools AR, Horstink MW (1998) Reliance on external cues for movement initiation in Parkinson’s disease. Evidence from movement-related potentials. Brain 121 (Pt 1): 167–177.
[16]  Hausdorff JM, Balash J, Giladi N (2003) Effects of cognitive challenge on gait variability in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 16: 53–58.
[17]  Uc EY, Rizzo M, Anderson SW, Sparks JD, Rodnitzky RL, et al. (2006) Driving with distraction in Parkinson disease. Neurology 67: 1774–1780.
[18]  Cools R, Miyakawa A, Sheridan M, D’Esposito M (2010) Enhanced frontal function in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 133: 225–233.
[19]  Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Collins P, Van Denderen JC, Everitt BJ, et al. (2001) Differential effects of 6-OHDA lesions of the frontal cortex and caudate nucleus on the ability to acquire an attentional set. Cereb Cortex 11: 1015–1026.
[20]  Brown RG, Marsden CD (1988) Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 111 (Pt 2): 323–345.
[21]  Lewis GN, Byblow WD, Walt SE (2000) Stride length regulation in Parkinson’s disease: the use of extrinsic, visual cues. Brain 123 (Pt 10): 2077–2090.
[22]  Hoehn MM, Yahr MD (1967) Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17: 427–442.
[23]  Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12: 189–198.
[24]  Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4: 561–571.
[25]  Weintraub D, Hoops S, Shea JA, Lyons KE, Pahwa R, et al. (2009) Validation of the questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 24: 1461–1467.
[26]  Fahn S ER, Committee MotUD, editor (1987) Unified Parkinson’s disease rating scale: McMillan Health Care Information.
[27]  Seymour B, Daw ND, Roiser JP, Dayan P, Dolan R (2012) Serotonin selectively modulates reward value in human decision-making. J Neurosci 32: 5833–5842.
[28]  Niv Y (2007) Cost, benefit, tonic, phasic: what do response rates tell us about dopamine and motivation? Ann N Y Acad Sci 1104: 357–376.
[29]  Graef S, Biele G, Krugel LK, Marzinzik F, Wahl M, et al. (2010) Differential influence of levodopa on reward-based learning in Parkinson’s disease. Front Hum Neurosci 4: 169.
[30]  Knowlton BJ, Mangels JA, Squire LR (1996) A neostriatal habit learning system in humans. Science 273: 1399–1402.
[31]  Czernecki V, Pillon B, Houeto JL, Pochon JB, Levy R, et al. (2002) Motivation, reward, and Parkinson’s disease: influence of dopatherapy. Neuropsychologia 40: 2257–2267.
[32]  Frank MJ, Seeberger LC, O’Reilly R C (2004) By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306: 1940–1943.
[33]  Frank MJ, Samanta J, Moustafa AA, Sherman SJ (2007) Hold your horses: impulsivity, deep brain stimulation, and medication in parkinsonism. Science 318: 1309–1312.
[34]  Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (2006) Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature 442: 1042–1045.
[35]  Cunnington R, Iansek R, Bradshaw JL, Phillips JG (1995) Movement-related potentials in Parkinson’s disease. Presence and predictability of temporal and spatial cues. Brain 118 (Pt 4): 935–950.
[36]  Baker K, Rochester L, Nieuwboer A (2007) The immediate effect of attentional, auditory, and a combined cue strategy on gait during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 88: 1593–1600.
[37]  Monchi O, Petrides M, Mejia-Constain B, Strafella AP (2007) Cortical activity in Parkinson’s disease during executive processing depends on striatal involvement. Brain 130: 233–244.
[38]  Samuel M, Ceballos-Baumann AO, Blin J, Uema T, Boecker H, et al. (1997) Evidence for lateral premotor and parietal overactivity in Parkinson’s disease during sequential and bimanual movements. A PET study. Brain 120 (Pt 6): 963–976.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133