|
内疚对中学生言语发散思维的影响:有调节的中介模型
|
Abstract:
通过对800名中学生进行问卷调查,本研究旨在探究内疚感与言语发散思维之间的关系,并揭示亲社会倾向在其中的中介作用以及认知需求的调节作用。研究结果显示,内疚感不仅能够直接显著地正向预测中学生的言语发散思维能力,还能通过亲社会倾向间接地影响他们的言语发散思维。此外,个体的认知需求调节了这种间接影响,显示出在低认知需求的个体中,内疚感对亲社会倾向的正向预测作用更为突出。这些发现表明,内疚感能够通过激发亲社会动机来提升个体的发散思维水平,并为如何有效地培养中学生的创造性思维提供了有益的参考。
This study endeavors to delve into the intricate relationship between guilt and verbal divergent thinking among 800 middle school students through a comprehensive questionnaire survey. Its objectives encompass elucidating the mediating influence of prosocial tendencies and the moderating effect of cognitive needs. The outcomes reveal a dual-faceted impact: guilt directly and substantially predicts the verbal divergent thinking prowess of these students, while also exerting an indirect influence through fostering prosocial tendencies. Furthermore, individual cognitive needs serve as a crucial moderator, amplifying the positive predictive link between guilt and prosocial tendencies, particularly among those with lower cognitive needs. These revelations underscore the potential of guilt to bolster divergent thinking by nurturing prosocial motivations, offering profound insights into strategies for fostering creative thinking among middle school students.
[1] | 程黎, 王寅枚, 何聪, 段达娜, 庞亚男(2017). 流动与城市超常儿童创造力发展的比较研究. 中国特殊教育, (11), 57-63. |
[2] | 丁芳, 周鋆, 胡雨(2014). 初中生内疚情绪体验的发展及其对公平行为的影响. 心理科学, 37(5), 1154-1159. |
[3] | 方圆(2017). 大学生内疚感与亲社会倾向关系实证研究. 南阳师范学院学报, 16(2), 61-64. |
[4] | 郝娜, 崔丽莹(2022). 补偿他人还是保护自己?内疚与羞耻情绪对合作行为的影响差异. 心理科学进展, 30(7), 1626-1636. |
[5] | 郝宁, 汤梦颖(2017). 动机对创造力的作用: 研究现状与展望. 华东师范大学学报(教育科学版), 35(4), 107-114, 138. |
[6] | 何华容, 丁道群(2016). 内疚: 一种有益的负性情绪. 心理研究, 9(1), 3-8. |
[7] | 侯志军, 姚佳, 王正元(2014). 大学生亲社会倾向与志愿动机的关系研究. 当代青年研究, (2), 92-96. |
[8] | 胡卫平, 王博韬, 段海军, 程丽芳, 周寰, 李晶晶(2015). 情绪影响创造性认知过程的神经机制. 心理科学进展, 23(11), 1869-1878. |
[9] | 寇彧, 洪慧芳, 谭晨, 李磊(2007). 青少年亲社会倾向量表的修订. 心理发展与教育, 23(1), 112-117. |
[10] | 李成(2021). 颜色和情绪对创造性问题解决的影响. 硕士学位论文. 保定: 河北大学. |
[11] | 李阳, 白新文(2015). 善心点亮创造力: 内部动机和亲社会动机对创造力的影响. 心理科学进展, 23(2), 175-181. |
[12] | 卢家楣, 刘伟, 贺雯, 卢盛华(2002). 情绪状态对学生创造性的影响. 心理学报, 34(4), 381-386. |
[13] | 罗晓路, 林崇德(2006). 大学生心理健康、创造性人格与创造力关系的模型建构. 心理科学, 29(5), 1031-1034. |
[14] | 罗彦红, 石文典(2010). 创造力与人格关系的研究评述. 心理学探新, 30(2), 76-78. |
[15] | 孙翠翠(2018). 自恋对群际亲社会行为的影响. 硕士学位论文. 西安: 陕西师范大学. |
[16] | 王博韬, 魏萍(2021). 道德情绪: 探寻道德与创造力关系的新视角. 心理科学进展, 29(2), 268-275. |
[17] | 王小凤, 占友龙, 燕良轼(2016). 中文版内疚和羞耻倾向量表的信效度检验. 中国临床心理学杂志, 24(5), 865-868. |
[18] | 吴洁清, 董勇燕, 周治金(2015). 教师创造性教学行为对中学生创造性问题解决的影响. 应用心理学, (3), 281-288. |
[19] | 薛贵, 董奇, 周龙飞, 张华, 陈传生(2001). 内部动机、外部动机与创造力的关系研究. 心理发展与教育, 17(1), 6-11. |
[20] | 杨秀木, 郭宏艳, 申正付, 周静, 赵龙(2017). 志愿动机与志愿行为: 亲社会倾向的中介作用. 蚌埠医学院学报, 42(7), 963-967. |
[21] | 尹素婷(2019). 认知需求与中学生创造力的关系: 父母参与的调节作用. 硕士学位论文. 济南: 山东师范大学. |
[22] | 张潮, 安彦名, 刘金蕙, 吕一超, 喻丰(2020). 敬畏情绪对创造力的影响: 认知灵活性的中介作用. 中国社会心理学评论, (2), 153-176, 241. |
[23] | 张克, 杜秀敏, 董晓飞(2018). 社会动机与“自我-他人”对创造性表现的影响. 心理与行为研究, 16(3), 414-420. |
[24] | 张鹏程, 丁梦夏, 王灿明(2017). 积极情绪体验对创造力影响. 心理与行为研究, 15(5), 613-618. |
[25] | 张田, 傅宏(2016). 得到宽恕与否对冒犯者后续行为的影响: 内疚与共情的作用. 心理与行为研究, 14(1), 100-105. |
[26] | 张晓洁, 刘新梅(2018). 团队亲社会动机与团队创造力: 一个双中介模型. 科技进步与对策, 35(11), 139-146. |
[27] | 赵源, 王岳, 秦川, 宋欢(2021). 积极情绪和消极情绪对创造力表现的影响. 阿坝师范学院学报, 38(1), 121-128. |
[28] | 周详, 杨治良, 郝雁丽(2007). 理性学习的局限: 道德情绪理论对道德养成的启示. 道德与文明, (3), 57-60. |
[29] | 宗利永, 庄子匀, 陈敬良(2016). 人格特质对虚拟社区知识共享的影响: 以内在动机为调节变量. 科技和产业, 16(5), 41-46. |
[30] | Baas, M., Roskes, M., Koch, S., Cheng, Y., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2019). Why Social Threat Motivates Malevolent Creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45, 1590-1602. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219838551 |
[31] | Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The Need for Cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116 |
[32] | Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Feng Kao, C. (1984). The Efficient Assessment of Need for Cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 306-307. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13 |
[33] | Carlo, G., & Randall, B. A. (2002). The Development of a Measure of Prosocial Behaviors for Late Adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 31-44. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1014033032440 |
[34] | Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP Scale: A New Measure of Guilt and Shame Proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 947-966. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022641 |
[35] | Covert, M. V., Tangney, J. P., Maddux, J. E., & Heleno, N. M. (2003). Shame-Proneness, Guilt-Proneness, and Interpersonal Problem Solving: A Social Cognitive Analysis. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.22.1.1.22765 |
[36] | De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic Tone and Activation Level in the Mood-Creativity Link: Toward a Dual Pathway to Creativity Model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 739-756. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739 |
[37] | De Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral Sentiments and Cooperation: Differential Influences of Shame and Guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1025-1042. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600980874 |
[38] | Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., & Pretz, J. (1998). Can the Promise of Reward Increase Creativity? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 704-714. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.704 |
[39] | Forgeard, M. J. C., & Mecklenburg, A. C. (2013). The Two Dimensions of Motivation and a Reciprocal Model of the Creative Process. Review of General Psychology, 17, 255-266. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032104 |
[40] | Fu, H., & Zhang, Z. (2022). The Inhibitory Effect of Moral Emotions on Malevolent Creativity: Exploring the Mediation Role of Emotional Valence and Prosocial Behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, Article 945848. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.945848 |
[41] | Gasper, K. (2003). When Necessity Is the Mother of Invention: Mood and Problem Solving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 248-262. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-1031(03)00023-4 |
[42] | Haidt, J. (2001). The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.4.814 |
[43] | Lapan, C., & Boseovski, J. J. (2017). The Effects of Guilt on Preschoolers’ Cognitive Flexibility and Inhibition. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 41, 95-102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.06.004 |
[44] | Li, Y., Xiao, X., Ma, W., Jiang, J., Qiu, J., & Zhang, Q. (2013). Electrophysiological Evidence for Emotional Valence and Competitive Arousal Effects on Insight Problem Solving. Brain Research, 1538, 61-72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.09.021 |
[45] | Lopez, F. G., Gover, M. R., Leskela, J., Sauer, E. M., Schirmer, L., & Wyssmann, J. (1997). Attachment Styles, Shame, Guilt, and Collaborative Problem‐Solving Orientations. Personal Relationships, 4, 187-199. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1997.tb00138.x |
[46] | Pessoa, L., & Engelmann, J. B. (2010). Embedding Reward Signals into Perception and Cognition. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, Article 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2010.00017 |
[47] | Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The Standard Definition of Creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 92-96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.650092 |
[48] | Saroglou, V., & Dupuis, J. (2006). RESEARCH: Being Buddhist in Western Europe: Cognitive Needs, Prosocial Character, and Values. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 16, 163-179. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327582ijpr1603_2 |
[49] | Sellier, A. L., & Dahl, D. W. (2015). The Light Side of Creativity: An Honesty Mindset Can Boost Creativity. ACR North American Advances. |
[50] | Shen, W., & Liu, C. (2012). Critical Review on Psychological Studies on Moral Hypocrisy. Advances in Psychological Science, 20, 745-756. https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1042.2012.00745 |
[51] | Shen, W., Yuan, Y., Yi, B., Liu, C., & Zhan, H. (2019). A Theoretical and Critical Examination on the Relationship between Creativity and Morality. Current Psychology, 38, 469-485. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9613-9 |
[52] | Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Martinez, A. G. (2014). Two Faces of Shame: The Roles of Shame and Guilt in Predicting Recidivism. Psychological Science, 25, 799-805. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613508790 |
[53] | Vaish, A. (2018). The Prosocial Functions of Early Social Emotions: The Case of Guilt. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 25-29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.008 |
[54] | Xiao, X., Zhan, Y., & Zhong, Y. (2022). Parental Attachment and Adolescent Internet Morality: A Moderated Mediation Model. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 1-21. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2020.1852922 |
[55] | Yun, T., Danna, L., Ting, D., & Xie, M. (2021). The Influence of Guilt Emotion on Cognitive Flexibility. In B. Bozoğlan, & M. Dixit (Eds.), 2021 2nd International Conference on Mental Health and Humanities Education (ICMHHE 2021)(pp. 579-583). Atlantis Press. https://doi.org/10.2991/assehr.k.210617.142 |