全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

类SNARC效应的特殊性及其内在机制
The Peculiarities of the SNARC-Like Effect and Its Internal Mechanism

DOI: 10.12677/ap.2024.146395, PP. 171-177

Keywords: 类SNARC,模拟量,近似数量系统,先天性,三路径模型
SNARC-Like
, Analogue, Approximate Number System, Innate, Three-Path Model

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

数字和空间之间存在着联结现象。这种现象不仅出现在离散数字中,在含有模拟量的材料中也有所体现,本文将模拟量信息中出现的这种现象命名为类SNARC效应(SNARC-like effect)。对于类SNARC效应的发生机制,研究者认为近似数量系统在类SNARC效应中起到重要作用,并在双路径模型的基础上,将类SNARC效应纳入其中,提出了三路径理论模型。关于两种效应之间的具体区别,仍需进一步神经机制研究的探索。
There is a connection between digital and space. This phenomenon is not only seen in discrete numbers, but also in materials containing analog quantities, and this phenomenon is named SNARC-like effect in this paper. As for the mechanism of SNARC-like effects, the researchers believe that the approximate number system plays an important role in the SNARC-like effects, and on the basis of the two-path model, the SNARC-like effects are included in it, and a three-path theoretical model is proposed. The specific differences between the two effects still need to be explored by further neurological studies.

References

[1]  戴隆农, 潘运(2021). 数字-空间联结的内在机制: 基于工作记忆的视角. 心理科学, 44(4), 793-799.
[2]  邓之君, 吴慧中, 陈英和(2017). 数字空间联结的工作记忆机制. 心理科学进展, 25(9), 1492-1502.
[3]  定险峰, 靖桂芳, 徐成(2010). SNARC效应起源的实验研究. 心理科学, 33(5), 1258-1261.
[4]  胡林成, 熊哲宏(2011). 刺激模拟量的空间表征: 面积和亮度的类SNARC效应. 心理科学, 34(1), 58-62.
[5]  胡林成, 熊哲宏(2016). 符号数量和非符号数量的空间表征: 5岁儿童的SNARC效应和距离效应. 心理科学, 39(2), 364-370.
[6]  康武, 杨敏, 王丽平(2013). SNARC效应: 现状, 理论及建议. 心理科学, 36(5), 1242-1248.
[7]  刘超, 傅小兰(2004). 不同注意条件下大数与小数的加工差异. 心理学报, 36(3), 307-314.
[8]  司继伟, 周超, 张传花, 仲蕾蕾(2013). 不同加工深度非符号数量信息的SNARC: 眼动证据. 心理学报, 45(1), 11-22.
[9]  王静, 陈英, 曹仕莹(2011). 类比数量表征的线索: 离散量还是连续量. 心理发展与教育, 27(1), 1-8.
[10]  王岚(2018). 音高的SNARC效应研究及其在识谱教学中的应用. 硕士学位论文, 西安: 西安音乐学院.
[11]  杨林霖, 张志杰, 顾艳艳, 周文杰(2013). 空间-时间联合编码效应: 来自手动和眼动证据. 心理科学, 36(6), 1347-1354.
[12]  尹月阳(2019). 符号数量系统与近似数量系统之间的双向映射机制. 博士学位论文, 长春: 吉林大学.
[13]  于晓, 戚玥, 蒋家丽, 陈英和(2022). 空间-数字联结如何发展: 符号和非符号SNARC效应的分离机制. 见 第二十四届全国心理学学术会议摘要集(pp. 48-50). 北京林业大学人文社会科学学院, 北京师范大学心理学部发展心理研究院.
[14]  Abrahamse, E., Van Dijck, J., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the Answer in Space: The Mental Whiteboard Hypothesis on Serial Order in Working meMory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
[15]  Barth, H. C., La Mont, K., Lipton, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Abstract Number and Arithmetic in Preschool Children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 14116-14121.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0505512102
[16]  Bonato, M., & Umiltà, C. (2014). Heterogeneous Timescales Are Spatially Represented. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article No. 542.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00542
[17]  Cantlon, J. F., Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the Number Domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 83-91.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.007
[18]  Cohen, K. R., Henik, A., & Walsh, V. (2007). Small Is Bright and Big Is Dark in Synaesthesia. Current Biology, 17, R834-R835.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.048
[19]  Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122, 371-396.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
[20]  Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is Numerical Comparison Digital? Analogical and Symbolic Effects in Two-Digit Number Comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626-641.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.16.3.626
[21]  Di Giorgio, E., Lunghi, M., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., Dalla Barba, B., Vallortigara, G., & Simion, F. (2019). A Mental Number Line in Human Newborns. Developmental Science, 22, 1-10.
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12801
[22]  Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core Systems of Number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307-314.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
[23]  Felisatti, A., Ranzini, M., & Blini, E. (2022). Effects of Attentional Shifts along the Vertical Axis on Number Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Optokinetic Stimulation. Cognition, 221, Article ID: 104991.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104991
[24]  Fias, W., & Dijck, J.P. V. (2016). The Temporary Nature of Number-Space Interactions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 33-40.
https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000071
[25]  Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2009). How to Cook a SNARC: Number Placement in Text Rapidly Changes Spatial-Numerical Associations. Brain and Cognition, No. 3, 333-336.
[26]  Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a Common Processing Architecture Underlying Simon and SNARC Effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 659-673.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440540000112
[27]  Hurewitz, F., Gelman, R., & Schnitzer, B. (2006). Sometimes Area Counts More than Number. PNAS, USA, 103, 19599-19604.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609485103
[28]  Ito, E., Hatta, T., Ito, Y., Kogure, T., & Watanabe, H. (2004). Performance of Verbal Fluency Tasks in Japanese Healthy Adults: Effect of Gender, Age and Education on the Performance. Japanese Journal of Neuropsychology, 20, 254-263.
[29]  Izard, V. R., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn Infants Perceive Abstract Numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 10382-10385.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812142106
[30]  Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The Universal SNARC Effect: The Association between Number Magnitude and Space Is Amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52, 187-194.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187
[31]  Proctor, R., & Cho, Y. (2006). Polarity Correspondence: A General Principle for Performance of Speeded Binary Classification Tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416-442.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416
[32]  Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial Representation of Pitch Height: The SMARC Effect. Cognition, 99, 113-129.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.004
[33]  Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading Habits for both Words and Numbers Contribute to the SNARC Effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 328-331.
https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.328
[34]  Van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A Working Memory Account for Spatial Numerical Associations. Cognition, 119, 114-119.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013
[35]  Van Dijck, J. P., Gevers, W., & Fias, W. (2009). Numbers Are Associated with Different Types of Spatial Information Depending on the Task. Cognition, 113, 248-253.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.005
[36]  Van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing Access to Number Magnitude: A Study of the SNARC Effect in 7-to 9-Year-Olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 99-113.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133