|
类SNARC效应的特殊性及其内在机制
|
Abstract:
数字和空间之间存在着联结现象。这种现象不仅出现在离散数字中,在含有模拟量的材料中也有所体现,本文将模拟量信息中出现的这种现象命名为类SNARC效应(SNARC-like effect)。对于类SNARC效应的发生机制,研究者认为近似数量系统在类SNARC效应中起到重要作用,并在双路径模型的基础上,将类SNARC效应纳入其中,提出了三路径理论模型。关于两种效应之间的具体区别,仍需进一步神经机制研究的探索。
There is a connection between digital and space. This phenomenon is not only seen in discrete numbers, but also in materials containing analog quantities, and this phenomenon is named SNARC-like effect in this paper. As for the mechanism of SNARC-like effects, the researchers believe that the approximate number system plays an important role in the SNARC-like effects, and on the basis of the two-path model, the SNARC-like effects are included in it, and a three-path theoretical model is proposed. The specific differences between the two effects still need to be explored by further neurological studies.
[1] | 戴隆农, 潘运(2021). 数字-空间联结的内在机制: 基于工作记忆的视角. 心理科学, 44(4), 793-799. |
[2] | 邓之君, 吴慧中, 陈英和(2017). 数字空间联结的工作记忆机制. 心理科学进展, 25(9), 1492-1502. |
[3] | 定险峰, 靖桂芳, 徐成(2010). SNARC效应起源的实验研究. 心理科学, 33(5), 1258-1261. |
[4] | 胡林成, 熊哲宏(2011). 刺激模拟量的空间表征: 面积和亮度的类SNARC效应. 心理科学, 34(1), 58-62. |
[5] | 胡林成, 熊哲宏(2016). 符号数量和非符号数量的空间表征: 5岁儿童的SNARC效应和距离效应. 心理科学, 39(2), 364-370. |
[6] | 康武, 杨敏, 王丽平(2013). SNARC效应: 现状, 理论及建议. 心理科学, 36(5), 1242-1248. |
[7] | 刘超, 傅小兰(2004). 不同注意条件下大数与小数的加工差异. 心理学报, 36(3), 307-314. |
[8] | 司继伟, 周超, 张传花, 仲蕾蕾(2013). 不同加工深度非符号数量信息的SNARC: 眼动证据. 心理学报, 45(1), 11-22. |
[9] | 王静, 陈英, 曹仕莹(2011). 类比数量表征的线索: 离散量还是连续量. 心理发展与教育, 27(1), 1-8. |
[10] | 王岚(2018). 音高的SNARC效应研究及其在识谱教学中的应用. 硕士学位论文, 西安: 西安音乐学院. |
[11] | 杨林霖, 张志杰, 顾艳艳, 周文杰(2013). 空间-时间联合编码效应: 来自手动和眼动证据. 心理科学, 36(6), 1347-1354. |
[12] | 尹月阳(2019). 符号数量系统与近似数量系统之间的双向映射机制. 博士学位论文, 长春: 吉林大学. |
[13] | 于晓, 戚玥, 蒋家丽, 陈英和(2022). 空间-数字联结如何发展: 符号和非符号SNARC效应的分离机制. 见 第二十四届全国心理学学术会议摘要集(pp. 48-50). 北京林业大学人文社会科学学院, 北京师范大学心理学部发展心理研究院. |
[14] | Abrahamse, E., Van Dijck, J., Majerus, S., & Fias, W. (2014). Finding the Answer in Space: The Mental Whiteboard Hypothesis on Serial Order in Working meMory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. |
[15] | Barth, H. C., La Mont, K., Lipton, J., & Spelke, E. S. (2005). Abstract Number and Arithmetic in Preschool Children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 14116-14121. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0505512102 |
[16] | Bonato, M., & Umiltà, C. (2014). Heterogeneous Timescales Are Spatially Represented. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article No. 542. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00542 |
[17] | Cantlon, J. F., Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the Number Domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 83-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.007 |
[18] | Cohen, K. R., Henik, A., & Walsh, V. (2007). Small Is Bright and Big Is Dark in Synaesthesia. Current Biology, 17, R834-R835. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.048 |
[19] | Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122, 371-396. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371 |
[20] | Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is Numerical Comparison Digital? Analogical and Symbolic Effects in Two-Digit Number Comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 626-641. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-1523.16.3.626 |
[21] | Di Giorgio, E., Lunghi, M., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., Dalla Barba, B., Vallortigara, G., & Simion, F. (2019). A Mental Number Line in Human Newborns. Developmental Science, 22, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12801 |
[22] | Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core Systems of Number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307-314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002 |
[23] | Felisatti, A., Ranzini, M., & Blini, E. (2022). Effects of Attentional Shifts along the Vertical Axis on Number Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Optokinetic Stimulation. Cognition, 221, Article ID: 104991. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104991 |
[24] | Fias, W., & Dijck, J.P. V. (2016). The Temporary Nature of Number-Space Interactions. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 33-40. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000071 |
[25] | Fischer, M. H., Mills, R. A., & Shaki, S. (2009). How to Cook a SNARC: Number Placement in Text Rapidly Changes Spatial-Numerical Associations. Brain and Cognition, No. 3, 333-336. |
[26] | Gevers, W., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2005). Towards a Common Processing Architecture Underlying Simon and SNARC Effects. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17, 659-673. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440540000112 |
[27] | Hurewitz, F., Gelman, R., & Schnitzer, B. (2006). Sometimes Area Counts More than Number. PNAS, USA, 103, 19599-19604. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609485103 |
[28] | Ito, E., Hatta, T., Ito, Y., Kogure, T., & Watanabe, H. (2004). Performance of Verbal Fluency Tasks in Japanese Healthy Adults: Effect of Gender, Age and Education on the Performance. Japanese Journal of Neuropsychology, 20, 254-263. |
[29] | Izard, V. R., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn Infants Perceive Abstract Numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 10382-10385. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0812142106 |
[30] | Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The Universal SNARC Effect: The Association between Number Magnitude and Space Is Amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52, 187-194. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.52.3.187 |
[31] | Proctor, R., & Cho, Y. (2006). Polarity Correspondence: A General Principle for Performance of Speeded Binary Classification Tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416-442. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416 |
[32] | Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial Representation of Pitch Height: The SMARC Effect. Cognition, 99, 113-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.004 |
[33] | Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading Habits for both Words and Numbers Contribute to the SNARC Effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 328-331. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.328 |
[34] | Van Dijck, J. P., & Fias, W. (2011). A Working Memory Account for Spatial Numerical Associations. Cognition, 119, 114-119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.013 |
[35] | Van Dijck, J. P., Gevers, W., & Fias, W. (2009). Numbers Are Associated with Different Types of Spatial Information Depending on the Task. Cognition, 113, 248-253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.005 |
[36] | Van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing Access to Number Magnitude: A Study of the SNARC Effect in 7-to 9-Year-Olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 99-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001 |