全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

暴力伤害厌恶与主动性攻击关系的研究进展
Research Progress on the Relationship between Violent Harm Aversion and Proactive Aggression

DOI: 10.12677/ap.2024.144200, PP. 116-124

Keywords: 暴力伤害厌恶,主动性攻击,模拟想象
Violent Harm Aversion
, Proactive Aggression, Simulated Imagination

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

暴力伤害厌恶是一种重要的道德抑制因素,在指导道德判断选择和抑制攻击中发挥重要作用。本文总结了目前针对暴力伤害厌恶的研究方法的特点,提出通过观看视频诱发暴力伤害厌恶情绪的方法,即参与者通过观看包含暴力伤害场景的视频,并基于视频内容在脑海中模拟和想象视频中的场景或事件,以诱发其暴力伤害厌恶情绪。这种想象自己主动去攻击和伤害他人的方式属于主动性攻击,即在没有被他人激惹的情况下主动去做出伤害他人的行为,文中又继续探讨了暴力伤害厌恶与主动性攻击的关系。未来的研究可以开发出一个针对暴力伤害厌恶研究的实验范式,并深入探讨暴力伤害厌恶抑制主动性攻击的机制。
Violent harm aversion is recognized as a pivotal moral inhibitory construct, instrumental in shaping moral decision-making and mitigating aggressive dispositions. The present study systematically examines extant methodological approaches to the investigation of violent harm aversion and introduces a novel methodology for eliciting violent harm aversion via exposure to video content. Participants are exposed to video clips depicting scenes of violence or harm, with the aim of prompting cognitive simulation and visualization of the portrayed scenarios or events, thereby facilitating the induction of violent harm aversion. Envisioning oneself as initiating unprovoked attacks and inflicting harm upon others is categorized within the realm of proactive aggression, which entails the undertaking of harmful acts towards others absent any prior provocation. The present study explores the relationship between violent harm aversion and proactive aggression. Future research should aim to develop a refined paradigm for the exploration of violent harm aversion and to dissect the underpinning mechanisms through which violent harm aversion serves to curtail instances of proactive aggression.

References

[1]  茹怡珊(2020). 伤害厌恶对主动性攻击的抑制作用及其脑关联探究. 硕士学位论文, 重庆: 西南大学.
[2]  苏姝(2021). 伤害厌恶的攻击线索任务及其脑成像研究. 硕士学位论文, 重庆: 西南大学.
[3]  朱文凤(2019). 主动性攻击的脑基础. 博士学位论文, 重庆: 西南大学.
[4]  Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human Aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27-51.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231
[5]  Arora, S., Aggarwal, R., Sirimanna, P., Moran, A., Grantcharov, T., Kneebone, R., Sevdalis, N., & Darzi, A. (2011). Mental Practice Enhances Surgical Technical Skills: A Randomized Controlled Study. Annals of Surgery, 253, 265-270.
https://doi.org/10.1097/SLA.0b013e318207a789
[6]  Baldwin, J. R., Arseneault, L., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Fisher, H. L., Odgers, C. L., Ambler, A., Houts, R. M., Matthews, T., Ougrin, D., Richmond-Rakerd, L. S., Takizawa, R., & Danese, A. (2019). Adolescent Victimization and Self-Injurious Thoughts and Behaviors: A Genetically Sensitive Cohort Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 58, 506-513.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.07.903
[7]  Berman, J. Z., & Kupor, D. (2020). Moral Choice When Harming Is Unavoidable. Psychological Science, 31, 1294-1301.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620948821
[8]  Blair, R. (1995). A Cognitive Developmental Approach to Morality: Investigating the Psychopath. Cognition, 57, 1-29.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-P
[9]  Buss, A. H. (1966). Instrumentality of Aggression, Feedback, and Frustration as Determinants of Physical Aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 153-162.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0022826
[10]  Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
[11]  Carroll, A., McCarthy, M., Houghton, S., Sanders, O. E., & Zadow, C. (2018). Reactive and Proactive Aggression as Meaningful Distinctions at the Variable and Person Level in Primary School-Aged Children. Aggressive Behavior, 44, 431-441.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21763
[12]  Cecchetto, C., Lancini, E., Rumiati, R. I., & Parma, V. (2019). Women Smelling Men’s Masked Body Odors Show Enhanced Harm Aversion in Moral Dilemmas. Physiology & Behavior, 201, 212-220.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.01.007
[13]  Cen, Y., Su, S., Dong, Y., & Xia, L. X. (2022). Longitudinal Effect of Self-Control on Reactive-Proactive Aggression: Mediating Roles of Hostile Rumination and Moral Disengagement. Aggressive Behavior, 48, 583-594.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.22046
[14]  Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Understanding Disgust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, 62-76.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06369.x
[15]  Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A Review and Reformulation of Social Information-Processing Mechanisms in Children’s Social Adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74
[16]  Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1996). Social Information-Processing Mechanisms in Reactive and Proactive Aggression. Child Development, 67, 993-1002.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01778.x
[17]  Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Hauser, M. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2010). Serotonin Selectively Influences Moral Judgment and Behavior through Effects on Harm Aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 17433-17438.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1009396107
[18]  Cushman, F., Gray, K., Gaffey, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Simulating Murder: The Aversion to Harmful Action. Emotion, 12, 2-7.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025071
[19]  Denson, T. F., Pedersen, W. C., & Miller, N. (2006). The Displaced Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 1032-1051.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.1032
[20]  Dodge, K. A. (1993). Social-Cognitive Mechanisms in the Development of Conduct Disorder and Depression. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 559-584.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.44.1.559
[21]  Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-Information-Processing Factors in Reactive and Proactive Aggression in Children’s Peer Groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.6.1146
[22]  Dodge, K. A., & Crick, N. R. (1990). Social Information-Processing Bases of Aggressive Behavior in Children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 8-22.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167290161002
[23]  Dodge, K. A., & Godwin, J. (2013). Social-Information-Processing Patterns Mediate the Impact of Preventive Intervention on Adolescent Antisocial Behavior. Psychological Science, 24, 456-465.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457394
[24]  Foster, E. M., & Jones, D. E. (2005). The High Costs of Aggression: Public Expenditures Resulting from Conduct Disorder. American Journal of Public Health, 95, 1767-1772.
https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2004.061424
[25]  Gaesser, B. (2020). Episodic Mindreading: Mentalizing Guided by Scene Construction of Imagined and Remembered Events. Cognition, 203, Article ID: 104325.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104325
[26]  Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind Perception Is the Essence of Morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23, 101-124.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2012.651387
[27]  Hallford, D. J., Farrell, H., & Lynch, E. (2022). Increasing Anticipated and Anticipatory Pleasure through Episodic Thinking. Emotion, 22, 690-700.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000765
[28]  Heiphetz, L., & Young, L. (2014). A Social Cognitive Developmental Perspective on Moral Judgment. Behaviour, 151, 315-335.
https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003131
[29]  Hernandez-Lallement, J., Attah, A. T., Soyman, E., Pinhal, C. M., Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2020). Harm to Others Acts as a Negative Reinforcer in Rats. Current Biology, 30, 949-961.E7.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.01.017
[30]  Hou, Y., Li, X., & Xia, L. (2023). Common Mechanisms Underlying the Effect of Angry Rumination on Reactive and Proactive Aggression: A Moderated Mediation Model. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 39, 1035-1057.
https://doi.org/10.1177/08862605231201819
[31]  Lahat, A., Helwig, C. C., & Zelazo, P. D. (2012). Age-Related Changes in Cognitive Processing of Moral and Social Conventional Violations. Cognitive Development, 27, 181-194.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.02.002
[32]  Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in Facial Affect Recognition among Antisocial Populations: A Meta-Analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 454-465.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003
[33]  McDonald, M. M., Defever, A. M., & Navarrete, C. D. (2017). Killing for the Greater Good: Action Aversion and the Emotional Inhibition of Harm in Moral Dilemmas. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 770-778.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.06.001
[34]  Miller, R. M., Hannikainen, I. A., & Cushman, F. A. (2014). Bad Actions or Bad Outcomes? Differentiating Affective Contributions to the Moral Condemnation of Harm. Emotion, 14, 573-587.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035361
[35]  Nichols, S., & Mallon, R. (2006). Moral Dilemmas and Moral Rules. Cognition, 100, 530-542.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.07.005
[36]  Patil, I. (2015). Trait Psychopathy and Utilitarian Moral Judgement: The Mediating Role of Action Aversion. Journal of Cognitive Psychology), 27, 349-366.
https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2015.1004334
[37]  Rieger, N. S., Guoynes, C. D., Monari, P. K., Hammond, E. R., Malone, C. L., Marler, C. A., Harmon-Jones, E., Gendolla, G. H. E., Wright, R. A., & Schutter, D. J. L. G. (2022). Neuroendocrine Mechanisms of Aggression in Rodents. Motivation Science, 8, 81-105.
https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000260
[38]  Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2009). On the Nature of Medial Temporal Lobe Contributions to the Constructive Simulation of Future Events. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1245-1253.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0308
[39]  Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic Simulation of Future Events. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 39-60.
https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001
[40]  Schoeler, T., Duncan, L., Cecil, C. M., Ploubidis, G. B., Pingault, J., & Albarracín, D. (2018). Quasi-Experimental Evidence on Short-and Long-Term Consequences of Bullying Victimization: A Meta-Analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 1229-1246.
https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000171
[41]  Siegel, J. Z., & Crockett, M. J. (2013). How Serotonin Shapes Moral Judgment and Behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1299, 42-51.
https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12229
[42]  Valdés-Cuervo, A. A., Alcántar-Nieblas, C., Parra-Pérez, L. G., Torres-Acu?a, G. M., álvarez-Montero, F. J., & Reyes-Sosa, H. (2021). Unique and Interactive Effects of Guilt and Sympathy on Bystander Aggressive Defender Intervention in Cyberbullying: The Mediation of Self-Regulation. Computers in Human Behavior, 122, Article ID: 106842.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106842
[43]  Wang, Y., Cao, S., Zhang, Q., & Xia, L. X. (2020). The Longitudinal Relationship between Angry Rumination and Reactive-Proactive Aggression and the Moderation Effect of Consideration of Future Consequences-Immediate. Aggressive Behavior, 46, 476-488.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21913
[44]  Wrangham, R. W. (2018). Two Types of Aggression in Human Evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115, 245-253.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1713611115
[45]  Yildiz Durak, H., & Saritepeci, M. (2020). Examination of the Relationship between Cyberbullying and Cyber Victimization. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29, 2905-2915.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01768-4
[46]  Yu, H., Siegel, J. Z., & Crockett, M. J. (2019). Modeling Morality in 3-D: Decision-Making, Judgment, and Inference. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11, 409-432.
https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12382
[47]  Zhu, W., Wang, H., & Xia, L. (2019). An Experimental Task to Measure Proactive Aggression under Incentive Condition: A Reward-Interference Task. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 273-285.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.06.001

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133