全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...
Psychology  2024 

Adaptation of the CAD Scale to Japanese Parents: The Domains for Moral Violations in the CAD Triad Hypothesis

DOI: 10.4236/psych.2024.151005, PP. 58-76

Keywords: CAD Hypothesis, Moral Ethics, Factor Structure, Validity, Parents

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Background: The domains for moral emotions are still open to debate. Rozin et al.’s (1999) CAD triad hypothesis assumed selective, corresponding ties between three moral codes (community [C], autonomy [A], and divinity [D]) and three other-critical emotions (contempt [C], anger [A], and disgust [D]). Objective: To identify domains for violations of the big three moral codes in the CAD Scale and to examine the robustness of its 3-factor structure among a Japanese population. Methods: We used the data for a group of Japanese parents (n = 260) to whom the CAD Scale was distributed on the web. Each domain for the CAD hypothesis was analysed separately. Correlations between CAD Scale items and the total scores of the three domains, and alpha coefficients of the items belonging to each domain were calculated. Items with the highest correlation with another domain were deleted. A one-factor exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of the items for each domain for the 1-factor model were performed. Because 1-factor models did not reach satisfactory levels, we examined parcelled CFAs. Results: All items showed the highest correlation with the domain assumed by the CAD hypothesis except for items 8, 10, 15, 22, and 41. In the EFAs, all factor loadings showed higher than 0.33, and alpha coefficients of the C, A, and D domains were 0.937, 0.881, and 0.949, respectively. The CFAs of the 1-factor model for the C, A, and D domains did not show an acceptable fit. Parcelled models for each domain showed a perfect fit to the data in each domain of the CAD scale (χ2 = 0.000, CFI = 1.000, RMSEA = 0.000). Conclusion: These results support the 3-factor structure of the CAD Scale.

References

[1]  Bandalos, D. L., & Finney, S. J. (2001). Item Parceling Issues in Structural Equation Modeling. In G. A. Marcoulides, & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.). New Developments and Techniques in Structural Equation Modelling (pp. 269-296). Lawrence Erlbaum Associate Publisher.
[2]  Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., Nathan DeWall, C., & Zhang, L. (2007). How Emotion Shapes Behavior: Feedback, Anticipation, and Reflection, Rather than Direct Causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 167-203.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307301033
[3]  Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative Fit Indexes in Structural Models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
[4]  Cameron, C. D., Lindquist, K. A., & Gray, K. (2015). A Constructionist Review of Morality and Emotions: No Evidence for Specific Links between Moral Content and Discrete Emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19, 371-394.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314566683
[5]  Costello, A. B., & Osborne, J. W. (2005). Best Practices in Exploratory Factor Analysis: Four Recommendations for Getting the Most from Your Analysis. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 10, 1-9.
[6]  Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-Related Regulation: Sharpening the Definition. Child Development, 75, 334-339.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00674.x
[7]  Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions Revealed: Recognizing Faces and Feelings to Improve Communication and Emotional Life. Times Books/Henry Holt and Co.
[8]  Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face. Prentice Hall.
[9]  Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., O’Sullivan, M., Chan, A., Diacoyanni-Tarlatzis, I., Heider, K. et al. (1987). Universals and Cultural Differences in the Judgments of Facial Expressions of Emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 712-717.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.4.712
[10]  Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral Foundations Theory: The Pragmatic Validity of Moral Pluralism. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55-130). Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4
[11]  Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and Response-Focused Emotion Regulation: Divergent Consequences for Experience, Expression, and Physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224-237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
[12]  Gutierrez, R., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2007). Anger, Disgust, and Presumption of Harm as Reactions to Taboo-Breaking Behaviors. Emotion, 7, 853-868.
https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.853
[13]  Gutierrez, R., Giner-Sorolla, R., & Vasiljevic, M. (2012). Just an Anger Synonym? Moral Context Influences Predictors of Disgust Word Use. Cognition & Emotion, 26, 53-64.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2011.567773
[14]  Hada, A., Ohashi, Y., Usui, Y., & Kitamura, T. (2023). The Scale for Parent-to-Child Emotions: Validation Evidence among Japanese Parents. (Manuscript in preparation)
[15]  Haidt, J. (2003). The Moral Emotions. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 852-870). Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195126013.003.0045
[16]  Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When Morality Opposes Justice: Conservatives Have Moral Intuitions That Liberals May Not Recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98-116.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-007-0034-z
[17]  Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive Ethics: How Innately Prepared Intuitions Generate Culturally Variable Virtues. Daedalus, 133, 55-66.
http://www.jstor.org/stable/20027945
https://doi.org/10.1162/0011526042365555
[18]  Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, Psyche, and Culture: The Relationship between Disgust and Morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 107-131. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133369700900105
[19]  Hall, R. J., Snell, A. F., & Foust, M. S. (1999). Item Parceling Strategies in SEM: Investigating the Subtle Effects of Unmodeled Secondary Constructs. Organizational Research Methods, 2, 233-256. https://doi.org/10.1177/109442819923002
[20]  Heerdink, M. W., Koning, L. F., van Doorn, E. A., & van Kleef, G. A. (2019). Emotions as Guardians of Group Norms: Expressions of Anger and Disgust Drive Inferences about Autonomy and Purity Violations. Cognition and Emotion, 33, 563-578.
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1476324
[21]  Hutcherson, C. A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The Moral Emotions: A Social-Functionalist Account of Anger, Disgust, and Contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 719-737. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022408
[22]  Jensen, L. A. (1997). Culture Wars: American Moral Development across the Lifespan. Journal of Adult Development, 4, 107-121. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02510084
[23]  Jensen, L. A. (2008). Through Two Lenses: A Cultural-Developmental Approach to Moral Psychology. Developmental Review, 28, 289-315.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.11.001
[24]  Kenny, D. A., & McCoach, D. B. (2003). Effect of the Number of Variables on Measures of Fit in Structural Equation Modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 10, 333-351.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM1003_1
[25]  Kijima, N., Tanaka, E., Suzuki, N., Higuchi, H., & Kitamura, T. (2000). Reliability and Validity of the Japanese Version of the Temperament and Character Inventory. Psychological Reports, 86, 1050-1058. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3.1050
[26]  Kishton, J. M., & Widaman, K. F. (1994). Unidimensional versus Domain Representative Parceling of Questionnaire Items: An Empirical Example. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54, 757-765. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164494054003022
[27]  Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and Basic Psychological Processes—Toward a System View of Culture: Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 89-96.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.89
[28]  Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A Cultural Task Analysis of Implicit Independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 236-255.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015999
[29]  Landmann, H., & Hess, U. (2018). Testing Moral Foundation Theory: Are Specific Moral Emotions Elicited by Specific Moral Transgressions? Journal of Moral Education, 47, 34-47. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2017.1350569
[30]  Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To Parcel or Not to Parcel: Exploring the Question, Weighing the Merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151-173. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1
[31]  Little, T. D., Rhemtulla, M., Gibson, K., & Schoemann, A. M. (2013). Why the Items versus Parcels Controversy Needn’t Be One. Psychological Methods, 18, 285-300.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033266
[32]  Little, T. D., Rioux, C., Odejimi, O. A., & Stickley, Z. L. (2022). Parceling in Structural Equation Modeling: A Comprehensive Introduction for Developmental Scientists. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009211659
[33]  Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
[34]  Matsunaga, M. (2008). Item Parceling in Structural Equation Modeling: A Primer. Communication Methods and Measures, 2, 260-293.
https://doi.org/10.1080/19312450802458935
[35]  Meade, A. W., & Kroustalis, C. M. (2006). Problems with Item Parceling for Confirmatory Factor Analytic Tests of Measurement Invariance. Organizational Research Methods, 9, 369-403. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428105283384
[36]  Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Güler, E., Balliet, D., & Hofmann, W. (2017). Disgust and Anger Relate to Different Aggressive Responses to Moral Violations. Psychological Science, 28, 609-619. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617692000
[37]  Oda, R., & Sawada, K. (2021). Do Social Relationships with Those Who Witness Moral Transgression Affect the Sense of Guilt? Evolutionary Psychology, 19.
https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049211032576
[38]  Rioux, C., Stickley, Z. L., Odejimi, O. A., & Little, T. D. (2020). Item Parcels as Indicators: Why, When, and How to Use Them in Small Sample Research. In R. van de Schoot, & M. Miočević (Eds.), Small Sample Size Solutions: A Guide for Applied Researchers and Practitioners (pp. 203-214). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429273872-17
[39]  Rothbaum, F., Rosen, K., Ujiie, T., & Uchida, N. (2002). Family Systems Theory, Attachment Theory, and Culture. Family Process, 41, 328-350.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.41305.x
[40]  Royzman, E., Atanasov, P., Landy, J. F., Parks, A., & Gepty, A. (2014). CAD or MAD? Anger (Not Disgust) as the Predominant Response to Pathogen-Free Violations of the Divinity Code. Emotion, 14, 892-905. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036829
[41]  Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD Triad Hypothesis: A Mapping between Three Moral Emotions (Contempt, Anger, Disgust) and Three Moral Codes (Community, Autonomy, Divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 574-586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.574
[42]  Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. H. (2003). Evaluating the Fit of Structural Equation Models: Tests of Significance and Descriptive Goodness-of-Fit Measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8, 23-74.
[43]  Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1990). Culture and Moral Development. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development (pp. 130-204). Cambridge University Press. (Reprinted from “The Emergence of Morality in Young Children”, Jerome Kagan and Sharon Lamb (Eds.), Chicago & London: University of Chicago Press, 1987)
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173728.005
[44]  Song, C., Rui, X., & Xie, N. (2022). Justifications of Emotional Responses to Eliciting Situations: A Narratological Approach to the CAD Hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, Article 1019485. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1019485
[45]  Sunar, D., Cesur, S., Piyale, Z. E., Tepe, B., Biten, A. F., Hill, C. T., & Koç, Y. (2021). People Respond with Different Moral Emotions to Violations in Different Relational Models: A Cross-Cultural Comparison. Emotion, 21, 693-706.
https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000736
[46]  Tangney, J. P. (1991). Moral Affect: The Good, the Bad, and the Ugly. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598-607. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.4.598
[47]  Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and Guilt. Guilford Press.
[48]  Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral Emotions and Moral Behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345-372.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
[49]  Triandis, H. C. (1989). The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506

Full-Text

comments powered by Disqus

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133

WeChat 1538708413