|
反贫困视角下的短视行为研究综述
|
Abstract:
贫困心理的相关研究已经在心理学领域占据了重要地位,短视行为正是贫困心理负面内容中的突出问题。相对来说,贫困群体的短视行为更加明显,且这种行为表现还会因为情境不同而有所差异。作为跨期决策中的一种负向结果偏好,短视行为的个体会为了追求当下利益,忽略或放弃长远利益。本文通过对跨期决策以及短视行为相关文献进行整理,总结归纳了国内外最新研究成果,包括短视行为的内涵、表现、影响因素以及产生后果等。对短视行为未来的研究进行评述与展望,为巩固我国脱贫成果以及共同富裕提供一定的参考。
The research on poverty psychology has occupied an important position in the field of psychology, and short-sighted behavior is a prominent problem in the negative content of poverty psychology. Comparatively speaking, the short-sighted behavior of the poor group is more obvious, and the per-formance of this behavior will vary depending on the situation. As a negative outcome preference in intertemporal decision-making, individuals with short-sighted behavior will ignore or give up long-term interests in order to pursue immediate interests. This paper summarizes the latest re-search results at home and abroad by collating the literature on intertemporal decision-making and short-sighted behavior, including the connotation, manifestations, influencing factors and conse-quences of short-sighted behavior. This paper reviews and looks forward to the future research on short-sighted behavior, which provides a certain reference for consolidating the achievements of poverty alleviation and common prosperity in China.
[1] | 阿不来提江, 刘扬, 朱晓睿, 郑蕊, 梁竹苑, 饶俪琳, 吴斌, 李纾(2015). 自我对跨期决策的影响——基于个人-集体主义文化视角. 心理科学进展, 23(11), 1981-1990. |
[2] | 曹聪, 李乃和(2012). 跨期决策中的消费者自我控制行为研究. 上海管理科学, 34(2), 67-70. |
[3] | 陈希希, 何贵兵(2014). 压力使人短视? 来自跨期决策的证据. 应用心理学, 20(1), 3-10. |
[4] | 方迎风(2019). 行为视角下的贫困研究新动态. 经济学动态, (1), 131-144. |
[5] | 杭承政, 胡鞍钢(2017). “精神贫困”现象的实质是个体失灵——来自行为科学的视角. 国家行政学院学报, (4), 97-103, 147. |
[6] | 何清华, 李丹丹(2020). 中国儿童青少年跨期决策的发展与脑发育机制. 心理科学进展, 28(3), 381-389. |
[7] | 胡坚, 钱宥妮(2005). 中国消费者短视行为的理论与实证研究. 山西财经大学学报, 27(2), 5-10. |
[8] | 胡原, 曾维忠(2020). 人穷志短: 农村贫困与志向失灵——基于中国家庭追踪调查(CFPS)数据. 农业技术经济, (11), 96-109. |
[9] | 蒋元萍, 孙红月(2019). 情绪对跨期决策的影响. 心理科学进展, 27(9), 1622-1630. |
[10] | 金晶(2019). 跨期决策中自我控制的影响机制及干预办法研究. 心理月刊, 14(2), 1-2. |
[11] | 李爱梅, 王海侠, 孙海龙, 熊冠星, 杨韶丽(2018). “长计远虑”的助推效应: 怀孕与环境跨期决策. 心理学报, 50(8), 858-867. |
[12] | 李培林, 魏后凯(2016). 中国扶贫开发报告. 社会科学文献出版社. |
[13] | 梁竹苑, 刘欢(2011). 跨期选择的性质探索. 心理科学进展, 19(7), 959-966. |
[14] | 刘扬, 孙彦(2014). 行为决策中框架效应研究新思路——从风险决策到跨期决策,从言语框架到图形框架. 心理科学进展, 22(8), 1205-1217. |
[15] | 佘升翔, 郑小伟, 周劼, 杨姗姗(2016). 恐惧降低跨期选择的耐心吗?——来自行为实验的证据. 心理学探新, 36(1), 25-30. |
[16] | 宋锡妍, 程亚华, 谢周秀甜, 龚楠焰, 刘雷(2021). 愤怒情绪对延迟折扣的影响: 确定感和控制感的中介作用. 心理学报, 53(5), 456-468. |
[17] | 孙红月, 崔丽莹, 李丹(2016). 负折扣现象:跨期决策中的自我-他人决策差异. 心理科学, 39(4), 970-976. |
[18] | 谭洪业, 徐会奇(2015). 短视特征下的农村居民消费行为研究. 农业经济与管理, (4), 59-68. |
[19] | 王含, 程倩春(2019). 心理扶贫: 价值、困境及路径研究. 探索, (3), 171-181. |
[20] | 吴胜涛, 张建新(2007). 贫困与反贫困: 心理学的研究. 心理科学进展, (6), 987-992. |
[21] | 吴小菊, 陈俊芳, 符佳慧, 李纾, 梁竹苑(2020). 健康领域的跨期决策与健康行为. 心理科学进展, 28(11), 1926-1938. |
[22] | 谢治菊, 钟金娴(2020). “认知税”: 一个理解持续贫困的新视角. 华中农业大学学报(社会科学版), (6), 87-93, 164-165. |
[23] | 徐富明, 黄龙, 张慧, 相鹏, 刘腾飞, 李亚红(2020). 行为贫困陷阱的心理机制与管理对策: 基于认知与动机双视角. 心理科学进展, 28(5), 681-691. |
[24] | 徐岚, 陈全, 崔楠, 陆凯丽(2019). 享受当下, 还是留待未来?——时间观对跨期决策的影响. 心理学报, 51(1), 96-105. |
[25] | 杨鑫蔚, 何贵兵(2015). 主观幸福感对跨期决策的影响. 应用心理学, 21(3), 242-248. |
[26] | 张邦科, 邓胜梁(2012). 中国城乡居民消费的过度敏感性变异. 世界经济文汇, (5), 82-94. |
[27] | 周晶, 张加峰, 李智勇, 李晶(2020). 得失情境下的图形框架对跨期选择倾向的影响. 心理学探新, 40(2), 164-168. |
[28] | 朱信凯, 杨顺江(2001). 中国农户短视消费行为分析. 中国农村观察, (6), 17-22. |
[29] | Aguilar, P., Caballero, A., Sevilla-no, V., Fernández, I., Mu?oz, D., & Carrera, P. (2020). The Relationships between Economic Scarcity, Concrete Mindset and Risk Behavior: A Study of Nicaraguan Adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17, Article 3845. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17113845 |
[30] | Anand, P., & Lea, S. (2010). The Psychology and Behavioural Economics of Poverty. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32, 205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.01.001 |
[31] | Ayllón, S., & Fusco, A. (2017). Are Income Poverty and Perceptions of Financial Difficulties Dynamically Interrelated? Journal of Economic Psychology, 61, 103-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2017.03.008 |
[32] | Boyle, M. H., & Offord, D. R. (1986). Smoking, Drinking and Use of Illicit Drugs among Adolescents in Ontario: Prevalence, Patterns of Use and Sociodemographic Correlates. Canadian Medi-cal Association Journal, 135, 1113-1121. |
[33] | Brennan, S. L., Henry, M. J., Nicholson, G. C., Kotowicz, M. A., & Pasco, J. A. (2009). Socioeconomic Status and Risk Factors for Obesity and Metabolic Disorders in a Population-Based Sample of Adult Females. Preventive Medicine, 49, 165-171.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.06.021 |
[34] | Chowdry, H., Crawford, C., & Goodman, A. (2011). The Role of Attitudes and Behaviours in Explaining Socio-Economic Differences in Attainment at Age 16. Longitudinal and Life Course Studies, 2, 59-76.
https://doi.org/10.14301/llcs.v2i1.141 |
[35] | Daniel, J. Z., Hickman, M., Macleod, J., Wiles, N., Lingford-Hughes, A., Farrell, M., Araya, R., Skapinakis, P., Haynes, J., & Lewis, G. (2009). Is Socioeconomic Status in Early Life Associated with Drug Use? A Systematic Review of the Evidence. Drug and Alcohol Review, 28, 142-153. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2008.00042.x |
[36] | do Nascimento, J. C. (2021). The Personal Wealth Importance to the Intertemporal Choice. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications, 565, 125559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physa.2020.125559 |
[37] | Droomers, M., Schrijvers, C. T., Stronks, K., van de Mheen, D., & Mackenbach, J. P. (1999). Educational Differences in Excessive Alcohol Consumption: The Role of Psychosocial and Mate-rial Stressors. Preventive Medicine, 29, 1-10.
https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0496 |
[38] | Droomers, M., Schrijvers, C. T., Van de Mheen, H., & Mackenbach, J. P. (1998). Educational Differences in Leisure-Time Physical Inactivity: A Descriptive and Explanatory Study. Social Science & Medicine, 47, 1665-1676.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00272-X |
[39] | Everson, S. A., Maty, S. C., Lynch, J. W., & Kaplan, G. A. (2002). Epidemiologic Evidence for the Relation between Socioeconomic Status and Depression, Obesity, and Diabetes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 891-895.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00303-3 |
[40] | Faralla, V., Novarese, M., & Di Giovinazzo, V. (2021). Replica-tion: Framing Effects in Intertemporal Choice with Children. Journal of Economic Psychology, 82, Article ID: 102345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2020.102345 |
[41] | Franco-Watkins, A. M., Mattson, R. E., & Jackson, M. D. (2016). Now or Later? Attentional Processing and Intertemporal Choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 29, 206-217. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1895 |
[42] | Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O’donoghue, T. (2002). Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351-401. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.40.2.351 |
[43] | Geoffrey Fisher (2021). Intertemporal Choices Are Causally Influenced by Fluctuations in Visual Attention. Management Science, 67, 4961-4981. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2020.3732 |
[44] | Haushofer, J. & Fehr, E. (2014). On the Psychology of Poverty. Science, 344, 862-867.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232491 |
[45] | Haushofer, J., Jain, P., Musau, A., & Ndetei, D. (2021). Stress May In-crease Choice of Sooner Outcomes, But Not Temporal Discounting. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 183, 377-396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2020.12.024 |
[46] | Hugo, R.-H. & Edith, T.-P. (2021). Descuento por demora: Aplicaciones a la promoción de la salud, prevención y tratamiento de enfermedades. |
[47] | Janzen, S. A., Magnan, N., Sharma, S., & Thompson, W. M. (2017). Aspirations Failure and Formation in Rural Nepal. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 139, 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.04.003 |
[48] | Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High Income Improves Evaluation of Life But Not Emotional Well-Being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 16489-16493.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011492107 |
[49] | Kim, S., Hwang, J., & Lee, D. (2008). Prefrontal Coding of Temporally Discounted Values during Intertemporal Choice. Neuron, 59, 161-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.010 |
[50] | Lampe, I., & Würtenberger, D. (2020). Loss Aversion and the De-mand for Index Insurance. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 180, 678-693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2019.10.019 |
[51] | Legleye, S., Janssen, E., Beck, F., Chau, N., & Khlat, M. (2011). So-cial Gradient in Initiation and Transition to Daily Use of Tobacco and Cannabis during Adolescence: A Retrospective Cohort Study. Addiction, 106, 1520-1531.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03447.x |
[52] | Liu, J., Huang, F. B., Wang, Z. H., Shuai, C. M., & Li, J. X. (2020). Understanding the Role of Rural Poor’s Endogenous Impetus in Poverty Reduction: Evidence from China. Sustain-ability, 12, Article 2487. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12062487 |
[53] | Liu, L. L., Chen, X. J., Cui, J. F., Wang, J., Zhang, Y. B., Neumann, D. L., Shum, D. H. K., Wang, Y., & Chan, R. C. K. (2016). Age Differences in Delay Discounting in Chinese Adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 205-209.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.006 |
[54] | Livingstone, S. M., & Lunt, P. K. (1992). Predicting Personal Debt and Debt Repayment: Psychological, Social and Economic Determinants. Journal of Economic Psychology, 13, 111-134. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(92)90055-C |
[55] | Méjean, C., Macouillard, P., Péneau, S., Hercberg, S., & Castet-bon, K. (2013). Perception of Front-Of-Pack Labels According to Social Characteristics, Nutritional Knowledge and Food Purchasing Habits. Public Health Nutrition, 16, 392-402.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980012003515 |
[56] | Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of Gratification in Children. Science, 244, 933-938.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056 |
[57] | Mobley, L. R., Root, E. D., Finkelstein, E. A., Khavjou, O., Farris, R. P., & Will, J. C. (2006). Environment, Obesity, and Cardiovascular Disease Risk in Low-Income Women. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 30, 327-332.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2005.12.001 |
[58] | Myerson, J., Baumann, A. A., & Green, L. (2017). Individual Dif-ferences in Delay Discounting: Differences Are Quantitative with Gains, But Qualitative with Losses. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 30, 359-372.
https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1947 |
[59] | Pashardes, P. (1986). Myopic and forward Looking Behavior in a Dynamic Demand System. International Economic Review, 27, 387-397. https://doi.org/10.2307/2526511 |
[60] | Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The Behavioural Constellation of Deprivation: Causes and Consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600234X |
[61] | Schilbach, F., Schofield, H., & Mullainathan, S. (2016). The Psychological Lives of the Poor. American Economic Review, 106, 435-440. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.p20161101 |
[62] | Sen, A. (2006). Conceptualizing and Measuring Poverty. In D. B. Grusky, & R. Kanbur (Eds.), Poverty and Inequality (pp. 30-46). Stanford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1515/9780804767590-003 |
[63] | Spears, D. S. (2010). Economic Decision-Making in Poverty De-pletes Behavioral Control. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 11, 72-72. https://doi.org/10.2202/1935-1682.2973 |
[64] | Wuepper, D., & Lybbert, T. J. (2017). Perceived Self-Efficacy, Poverty, and Economic Development. Annual Review of Resource Economics, 9, 383-404. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-resource-100516-053709 |