Background: Binary or dichotomous thinking can lead to propensity for aggression throughout a person’s life being an essential part of a person’s mentality and cultural code. Both dichotomous thinking and aggressiveness negatively affect a person’s resistance to stress and it hinders an adequate stress coping style. Aim: The aim of this study is to make a comparative analysis of dependence of different types of anger on the dichotomy of thinking in different cultures. Method: A Japanese sample consisted of 226 university students (180 females) whereas a Turkey sample included a total of 243 university students (145 females) who participated in an online survey applying socio-demographic forms, State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory, the Dichotomous Thinking Inventory. Multigroup Confirmatory Factor Analyses (MGCFAs) were conducted to examine measurement invariance of the DTI and STAXI using structural equation modeling. Multiple regression analyses were examined to assess the degree to which such a cultural aspect as of dichotomous thinking predicted the different anger traits. Results: Results of the multiple regression analysis for State Anger and Trait Anger demonstrated that the effect of the country indicating State Anger to be higher in Turkey than in Japan. A noticeable negative effect of the country was also significant indicating Trait Anger to be higher in Japan than in Turkey. Furthermore, Japanese people had lower dichotomous thinking scores than Turkish people. Conclusions: The results confirmed previously proposed cultural differences, as well as opened up new avenues for exploring cultural pathways.
References
[1]
Akaike, H. (1974). A New Look at the Statistical Model Identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 6, 716-723. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705
[2]
Akutsu, S., Yamaguchi, A., Kim, M.-S., & Oshio, A. (2016). Self-Construals, Anger Regulation, and Life Satisfaction in the United States and Japan. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 768. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00768
[3]
Aliyev, K., Erkin, H. C., Sancakli, E., & Ilhan Ozgur, I. (2018). Validity and Reliability Study of Turkish Version of Dichotomous Thinking Inventory. Ankara: Ankara University.
[4]
Aliyev, K., & Senturk Cankorur, V. (2018). Need-Emotion Organization: An Essay. Crisis Journal, 26, 1-4.
[5]
Anderson, C. A., & Huesmann, L. R. (2003). Human Aggression: A Social-Cognitive View. In M. A. Hogg, & J. Cooper (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (pp. 296-323). Sage Publications.
[6]
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative Fit Indexes in Structural Models. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 238-246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
[7]
Boiger, M., Mesquita, B., Uchida, Y., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2013). Condoned or Condemned: The Situational Affordance of Anger and Shame in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 540-553. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213478201
[8]
Boxer, P., & Dubow, E. F. (2001). A Social-Cognitive Information Processing Model for School-Based Aggression Reduction and Prevention Programs: Issues for Research and Practice. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 10, 177-192. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0962-1849(01)80013-5
[9]
Buss, A. H. (1961). The Psychology of Aggression. John Wiley & Sons Inc. https://doi.org/10.1037/11160-000
[10]
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
[11]
De Leersnyder, J., Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2013). Cultural Regulation of Emotion: Individual, Relational, and Structural Sources. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 55. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00055
[12]
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion Regulation: Affective, Cognitive, and Social Consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281-291. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0048577201393198
[13]
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria versus New Alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
[14]
Ma, G. (2020). Islands and the World from an Anthropological Perspective. International Journal of Anthropology and Ethnology, 4, Article No. 12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41257-020-00038-x
[15]
Nagtegaal, M. H., & Rassin, E. (2004). The Usefulness of the Thought Suppression Paradigm in Explaining Impulsivity and Aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1233-1244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.12.007
[16]
Oshio, A. (2009). Development and Validation of the Dichotomous Thinking Inventory. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 37, 729-742. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2009.37.6.729
[17]
Oshio, A. (2012). An All-or-Nothing Thinking Turns into Darkness: Relations between Dichotomous Thinking and Personality Disorders. Japanese Psychological Research, 54, 424-429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00515.x
[18]
Ozkarar-Gradwohl, F. G., Narita, K., Montag, C., Panksepp, J., Davis, K. L., Yama, M., & Scherler, H. R. (2018). Cross-Cultural Affective Neuroscience Personality Comparisons of Japan, Turkey and Germany. Springer-Verlag. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-018-0074-2
[19]
Park, D. C., Lodi-Smith, J., Drew, L., Haber, S., Hebrank, A., Bischof, G. N., & Aamodt, W. (2014). The Impact of Sustained Engagement on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: The Synapse Project. Psychological Science, 25, 103-112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613499592
[20]
Pehlivanturk, B. (2012). Turkish-Japanese Relations: Turning Romanticism into Rationality. International Journal, 67, 101-117. https://doi.org/10.1177/002070201206700108
[21]
Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the Dimension of a Model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461-464. https://doi.org/10.1214/aos/1176344136
[22]
Siegman, A. W., & Smith, T. W. (1994). Anger, Hostility, and the Heart. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
[23]
Spielberger, C. D. (1996). State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory: Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
[24]
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural Model Evaluation and Modification: An Interval Estimation Approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, 173-180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4
[25]
Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and Advancement versus Sadness and Subjugation: The Effect of Negative Emotion Expressions on Social Status Conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 86-94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.86
[26]
Tone, E. B., & Davis, J. S. (2012). Paranoid Thinking, Suspicion, and Risk for Aggression: A Neurodevelopmental Perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 1031-1046. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579412000521
[27]
Vierikko, E., Pulkkinen, L., Kaprio, J., & Rose, R. J. (2006). Genetic and Environmental Sources of Continuity and Change in Teacher-Rated Aggression during Early Adolescence. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 308-320. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20117
[28]
Voulgaridou, I., & Kokkinos, C. M. (2015). Relational Aggression in Adolescents: A Review of Theoretical and Empirical Research. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 23, 87-97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2015.05.006
[29]
Yamaguchi, A., Kim, M.-S., Oshio, A., & Akutsu, S. (2017). The Role of Anger Regulation on Perceived Stress Status and Physical Health. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 240-245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.053
[30]
Zengin, A., & Yaman, U. (2018). Buddhist and Manichaeist Views on War on the Old Uyghur Turkish Texts. Journal of Turkish Language and Literature, 58, 175-216. https://doi.org/10.26650/TUDED425625