|
学前儿童耐挫心理的发展特点及对坚持行为的影响
|
Abstract:
为探讨学前儿童耐挫心理的发展特点及其对坚持行为的影响,以102名3~5岁儿童为被试,创设失败情景故事,要求被试听故事,回答相应问题。结果发现:1) 耐挫心理在3~4岁阶段飞速发展;2) 耐挫心理四因素的发展呈分离趋势,即乐观和可控在3~4岁阶段飞速发展,坚信在4~5岁阶段飞速发展;3) 坚信能正向预测学前儿童失败后的坚持行为。研究结果表明,学前期是耐挫心理飞速发展的时期,坚信是预测学前儿童坚持行为的重要因素。
To explore the development characteristics of perseverance in preschoolers and its prediction of persistence behavior, 102 preschoolers aged 3~5 participated in the research. Scenario stories about failure were created, and preschoolers were asked to listen to the stories and answer corresponding questions. The results showed that: 1) Perseverance developed rapidly at the age of 3~4. 2) Optimism and controllability developed rapidly at the age of 3~4, and conviction developed rapidly at the age of 4~5. 3) Conviction positively predicted persistence behavior of preschoolers after experiencing failure. These results reveal that preschool is a period of rapid development of perseverance, and conviction is an important factor in predicting preschoolers’ persistence behavior.
[1] | 白学军, 都旭, 牛宏伟, 郝嘉佳(2020). 耐挫心理结构的探索: 基于大学生群体的测量. 心理与行为研究, 18(5), 638-644. |
[2] | 都旭(2021). 学生耐挫心理的发展特点和神经基础. 博士学位论文, 天津: 天津师范大学. |
[3] | 都旭, 白学军, 刘颖, 郝嘉佳(2021). 成功经历能改善低耐挫大学生的挫折心理. 心理科学, 44(5), 1216-1223. |
[4] | 都旭, 郝坚, 方平(2017). 学前儿童对不同类型道德故事的内隐信息和主旨的理解. 学前教育研究, (12), 12-23. |
[5] | 冯江平(1991). 挫折心理学. 山西教育出版社. |
[6] | 刘丽英, 刘云艳(2011). 幼儿抗挫折能力的发展特点. 学前教育研究, (2), 41-45. |
[7] | 杨博茜, 康佳, 都旭, 冯萌萌, 白学军(2023). 高、低耐挫大学生对成功与失败事件的归因研究. 心理学进展, 13(1), 154-160. |
[8] | 邹敏(2011). 2-6岁在园幼儿受挫的缘由与反应. 学前教育研究, (6), 41-44. |
[9] | Chang, T. C., Lay, K. L., & Kung, M. J. (2016). On-Task Predictors for Preschoolers’ Persistence in Meeting Challenge. In 24th Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development. |
[10] | Dennis, T. (2006). Emotional Self-Regulation in Preschoolers: The Interplay of Child Approach Reactivity, Parenting, and Control Capacities. Developmental Psychology, 42, 84-97. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.1.84 |
[11] | Du, X., & Hao, J. (2018). Moral Stories Emphasizing Actors’ Negative Emotions toward Their Nonhelping Behavior Promote Preschoolers’ Helping Behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 168, 19-31.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.11.011 |
[12] | Du, X., Yuan, S., Liu, Y., & Bai, X. (2021). Reading Struggle Stories of Role Models Can Improve Students’ Growth Mindsets. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article ID: 747039. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.747039 |
[13] | Heyman, G. D., Dweck, C. S., & Cain, K. M. (1992). Young Children’s Vulnerability to Self-Blame and Helplessness: Relationship to Beliefs about Goodness. Child Development, 63, 401-415. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131488 |
[14] | Luster. T., & McAdoo, H. (1996). Family and Child Influences on Educational Attainment: A Secondary Analysis of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Data. Developmental Psychology, 32, 26-39. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.32.1.26 |
[15] | Nichols, O., Vaughn, B. E., Coppola, G., Shin, N., & Krzysik, L. (2020). Relations between Preschooler Social Competence and Coping Tactics during Resource-Based Conflicts. Review of Social Development, 4, 1051-1070.
https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12451 |
[16] | Rhodes, M., & Brickman, D. (2010). Preschoolers’ Responses to Social Comparisons Involving Relative Failure. Psychological Science, 10, 968-972. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02184.x |
[17] | Sebastián-Enesco, C., Guerrero, S., & Enesco, I. (2020). What Makes Children Defy Their Peers? Chinese and Spanish Preschoolers’ Decisions to Trust (or Not) Peer Consensus. Social Development, 2, 494-508.
https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12416 |
[18] | Weisberg, D. S., & Hopkins, E. J. (2020). Preschoolers’ Extension and Export of Information from Realistic and Fantastical Stories. Infant and Child Development, 4, e2182. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2182 |