|
未来乐观偏差:意志归因的时间不对称
|
Abstract:
人们倾向于对未来事件持有过于乐观的信念。一方面,它使人们对自己在过去基本失败领域的未来表现仍保持信心,另一方面使人们低估自身遇到风险的可能性而造成判断失误,不自觉推迟完成任务。对未来的过度自信是由一系列的动机和认知因素决定的,包括自我提升动机以及对过去和未来事件的认知。本文旨在探讨未来乐观偏差的产生原因及影响因素,并在此基础上进一步明确未来的研究方向。
People tend to hold overly optimistic beliefs about what lies ahead. On the one hand, it allows people to remain confident in their future performance in domains where they have largely failed in the past; on the other hand, it makes people underestimate the likelihood that they will encounter risks resulting in errors in judgment and unconscious delays in completing tasks. Overconfidence in the future is determined by a range of motivational and cognitive factors, including self-improvement motivation and perceptions of past and future events. The purpose of this paper is to explore the causes and influencing factors of future optimism bias and to further clarify future research directions based on them.
[1] | 岑延远(2016). 解释水平视角下的乐观偏差效应. 心理科学, 39(3), 553-558. |
[2] | 景娟娟(2020). 乐观偏差研究述评. 心理月刊, 15(18), 222-225. |
[3] | 刘肖岑, 桑标, 窦东徽(2011). 自我提升的利与弊: 理论、实证及应用. 心理科学进展, 19(6), 883-895. |
[4] | 许岳培, 贺雯(2019). 自我预测中乐观倾向的研究进展. 教育生物学杂志, 7(2), 102-116. |
[5] | 翟文文(2021). 自我威胁对乐观偏差的影响. 硕士学位论文, 金华: 浙江师范大学. |
[6] | Armor, D. A., & Sackett, A. M. (2006). Accuracy, Error and Bias in Predictions for Real versus Hypothetical Events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 583-600. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.583 |
[7] | Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (1998). Situated Optimism: Specific Outcome Expectancies and Self-Regulation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 309-379). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60386-X |
[8] | Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the Future: Autobiographical Planning and the Functionality of Mind-Wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1604-1611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.007 |
[9] | Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Willpower, Choice and Self-Control. In G. Loewenstein, D. Read, & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives on Intertemporal Choice (pp. 201-216). Russell SAGE. |
[10] | Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Oettingen, G. (2016). Pragmatic Prospection: How and Why People Think about the Future. Review of General Psychology, 20, 3-16. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000060 |
[11] | Bortolotti, L., & Antrobus, M. (2015). Costs and Benefits of Realism and Optimism. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28, 194-198. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000143 |
[12] | Brunnermeier, M. K., Papakonstantinou, F., & Parker, J. A. (2017). Optimal Time-Inconsistent Beliefs: Misplanning, Procrastination, and Commitment. Management Science, 63, 1318-1340. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2015.2360 |
[13] | Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the “Planning Fallacy”: Why People Underestimate Their Task Completion Times. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 366-381. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.366 |
[14] | Burns, P., McCormack, T., Jaroslawska, A., Fitzpatrick, A., McGourty, J., & Caruso, E. M. (2019). The Development of Asymmetries in Past and Future Thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 148, 272-288. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000464 |
[15] | Caruso, E. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). A Wrinkle in Time—Asymmetric Valuation of Past and Future Events. Psychological Science, 19, 796-801. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02159.x |
[16] | Chambers, J. R. (2003). Egocentrism, Event Frequency, and Comparative Optimism: When What Happens Frequently Is “More Likely to Happen to Me”. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1343-1356. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203256870 |
[17] | Dunning, D. (2007). Prediction: The Inside View. In E. T. Higgins, & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed., pp. 69-90). Guilford. |
[18] | Ent, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Embodied Free Will Beliefs: Some Effects of Physical States on Metaphysical Opinions. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 147-154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.001 |
[19] | Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “Holier than Thou”: Are Self-Serving Assessments Produced by Errors in Self- or Social Predicion? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861-875. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.861 |
[20] | Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the Future. Science, 317, 1351-1354. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1144161 |
[21] | Gilovich, T. (1983). Biased Evaluation and Persistence in Gambling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1110-1146. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.6.1110 |
[22] | Gilovich, T., Kerr, M., & Medvec, V. H. (1993). Effect of Temporal Perspective on Subjective Confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 552-560. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.552 |
[23] | Heck, P. R., & Krueger, J. I. (2020). Self-Enhancement Error Motivates Social Projection. Social Cognition, 38, 489-522. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2020.38.5.489 |
[24] | Helzer, E. G., & Gilovich, T. (2012). Whatever Is Willed Will Be: A Temporal Asymmetry in Attributions to Will. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1235-1246. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212448403 |
[25] | Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases (pp. 306-334). Cambridge University Press. |
[26] | Koehler, D. J., & Poon, C. S. K. (2006). Self-Predictions Overweight Strength of Current Intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 517-524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.003 |
[27] | Lavagnon, I., & Simon, F. (2022). Optimism Bias and World Bank Project Performance. The Journal of Development Studies, 58, 2604-2623. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220388.2022.2102901 |
[28] | Norcross, J. C., Ratzin, A. C., & Payne, D. (1989). Ringing in the New Year: The Change Processes and Reported Outcomes of Resolutions. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 205-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/0306-4603(89)90050-6 |
[29] | Nussbaum, S., Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (2006). Predicting the Near and Distant Future. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 152-161. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.2.152 |
[30] | Polivy, J., & Herman, P. C. (2002). If at First You Don’t Succeed: False Hopes of Self Change. American Psychologist, 57, 677-689. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.57.9.677 |
[31] | Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2010). People Believe They Have More Free Will than Others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 22469-22474. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1012046108 |
[32] | Rothbart, M., & Snyder, M. (1970). Confidence in the Prediction and Postdiction of an Uncertain Outcome. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 2, 38-43. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0082709 |
[33] | Savitsky, K., Medvec, V. H., Charlton, A. E., & Gilovich, T. (1998). “What, Me Worry?” Arousal, Misattribution and the Effect of Temporal Distance on Confidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 529-536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167298245008 |
[34] | Stephan, E., Shidlovski, D., & Heller, D. (2017). Distant Determination and Near Determinism: The Role of Temporal Distance in Prospective Attributions to Will. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 113-121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.06.006 |
[35] | Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal Construal. Psychological Review, 110, 403-421. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403 |
[36] | Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440-463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963 |
[37] | Van Boven, L., & Ashworth. (2007). Looking Forward, Looking Back: Anticipation Is More Evocative than Retrospection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 289-300. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.289 |