|
早年创伤经历与负性情绪对认知控制的交互影响
|
Abstract:
已有研究表明负性情绪刺激有助于冲突的解决,然而负性情绪刺激对冲突解决的影响是否会受到早年创伤经历的影响目前尚不清楚。本研究采用Flanker范式探究了早年经历与负性情绪对认知控制的交互影响。研究结果显示情绪主效应显著,正性图片条件下的反应时比中性图片条件下的反应时短,负性图片条件下的反应时大于个体在正性条件下的反应时。早年创伤经历主效应显著,高早年创伤经历个体反应时大于低早年创伤经历个体。
Previous studies have shown that negative emotional stimulation contributes to conflict resolu-tion. However, it remains unclear whether the impact of negative emotional stimulation on con-flict resolution is affected by early traumatic experiences. Using the Flanker paradigm, this study explored the interaction between early experiences and negative emotions on cognitive control. The results showed that the response time of positive images was shorter than that of neutral images, and the response time of negative images was longer than that of individuals under positive conditions. The main effect of early traumatic experiences was significant, and the reaction time of individuals with high early traumatic experiences was larger than that of individuals with low early traumatic experiences.
[1] | 傅文青, 姚树娇, 于宏华, 赵幸福, 李茹, 李英, 张艳青(2005). 儿童期创伤问卷在中国高校大学生中应用的信效度研究. 中国临床心理学杂志, 13(1), 40-42. |
[2] | 贾静, 刘昌(2008)影片片段诱发情绪的生理活动研究. 中国健康心理学杂志, 16(10), 1187-1189. |
[3] | 王振宏, 刘亚, 蒋长好(2013). 不同趋近动机强度积极情绪对认知控制的影响. 心理学报, 45(5), 546-555. |
[4] | 叶冬青, 姚捷, 董玛霞(2004). 儿童期不良经历的研究现状. 疾病控制杂志, 8(6), 483-485. |
[5] | Bradley, M. M, &Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and Motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581-607). Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511546396.025 |
[6] | Dennis, T. A., Chen, C., & McCandliss, B. D. (2008). Threat-Related Attentional Biases: An Analysis of Three Attention Networks. Depression and Anxiety, 25, E1-E10. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20308 |
[7] | Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one Year Later: Role of the Amygdala and the Medial Temporal Lobe Memory System in Retrieving Emotional Memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 2626-2631. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0409848102 |
[8] | Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The Effect of Emotion on Cue Utilization and the Organization of Behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047707 |
[9] | Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005) Cognitive Control Mechanisms Resolve Conflict through Cortical Amplification of Task-Relevant Information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784-1790. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1594 |
[10] | Finucane, A. M., & Power, M. J. (2010). The Effect of Fear on Attentional Processing in a Sample of Healthy Female. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 42-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.08.005 |
[11] | Gomez, P., Stahel, W. A., & Danuser, B. (2004). Respiratory Responses during Affective Picture Viewing. Biological Psychology, 67, 359-373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.013 |
[12] | Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2010). Modulation of Early Conflict Processing: N200 Responses to Emotional Words in a Flanker Task. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3661-3664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.021 |
[13] | Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2011). Emotion Triggers Executive Attention: Anterior Cingulated Cortex and Amygdala Responses to Emotional Words in a Conflict Task. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 198-208. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21012 |
[14] | Stoltenborgh, M., Baker-mans-Kranenburg, M. J., Alink, L. R., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The Prevalence of Child Maltreatment across the Globe: Review of a Series of Meta Analyses. Child Abuse Review, 24, 37-50.
https://doi.org/10.1002/car.2353 |