|
情绪启动对视觉工作记忆的影响
|
Abstract:
目的:本研究探讨情绪启动下男女大学生不同效价情绪下视觉工作记忆的情况。方法:采用实验法,通过E-prime软件编写实验程序。采用随机化原则选择大学生被试60名,男女生各30人,进行混合实验2*3设计,两要素分别是性别(男、女)、情绪类型(正性情绪、负性情绪、中性情绪),把被试的视觉工作记忆任务的反应时和正确率作为因变量。结果:1、以正确率为因变量:1) 性别类型的主效应差异显著(F = 7.87, p < 0.05);2) 情绪类型的主效应差异显著(F = 7.80, p < 0.05);3) 性别*情绪类型的交互作用差异显著(F = 8.08, p < 0.05)。2、以反应时为因变量:1) 性别的主效应差异显著(F = 9.35, p < 0.05);2) 情绪类型的主效应差异显著(F = 40.46, p < 0.05)。结论:1) 男生的视觉工作记忆再认判断反应时比女生的再认判断反应时短;2) 男生的视觉工作记忆再认判断的正确率大于女生。3) 所有被试正性情绪状态下的再认判断正确率最高;所有被试中性情绪状态下的再认判断的正确率中等;所有被试负性情绪状态下的再认判断正确率最差。4) 所有被试正性情绪状态下再认判断的反应时最短;所有被试负性情绪状态下再认判断的反应时中等;所有被试中性情绪状态下再认判断的反应时最长。
Objective: To investigate the visual working memory of male and female college students under different valence of emotion priming. Methods: Using the experimental method, E- Prime software was used to write the experimental program. 60 college students, 30 male and 30 female were randomly selected. A mixed experiment 2*3 design was adopted. The two factors were gender (male and female) and emotion type (positive emotion, negative emotion and neutral emotion), respectively. Results: 1. Accuracy rate was used as the dependent variable: 1) The main effect of gender type was significantly different (F = 7.87, p < 0.05); 2) The main effect of emotion type was significantly different (F = 7.80, p < 0.05); 3) Gender * emotion type interaction had significant difference (F = 8.08, p < 0.05); Taking response time as the dependent variable: 1) there was significant difference in the main effect between genders (F = 9.35, p < 0.05); 2) The main effect of emotion type was significantly different (F = 40.46, p < 0.05). Conclusions: 1) the cognitive response time of visual working memory in boys was shorter than that in girls; 2) The correct rate of visual working memory recognition in boys was higher than that in girls. 3) The correct rate of recognition judgment of all subjects in positive emotional state was the highest; The correct rate of recognition judgment of all subjects in neutral emotional state was medium; The correct rate of recognition judgment of all subjects in negative emotional state was the worst. 4) The reaction time of recognition judgment was the shortest in all subjects under positive emotional state; The reaction time of recognition judgment of all subjects in negative emotional state was medium; The reaction time of recognition judgment was the longest under neutral emotional state.
[1] | Klauer, K.C. and Musch, J. (2003) Affective Priming: Findings and Theories. Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion. Lawrence Erlbaum. |
[2] | 范碧玉. 情绪干扰和语义干扰下的启动效应——基于武夷学院大学生情感与认知两者的关系研究[J]. 哈尔滨学院学报, 2021, 40(3): 121-123. |
[3] | 郑希付. 焦虑情绪与启动情绪两种状态下的记忆信息处理[J]. 心理科学, 2005(2): 351-355. |
[4] | 贺玲姣. 不同声刺激下的情绪反应与识别[D]: [硕士学位论文]. 杭州: 浙江大学, 2013. |
[5] | 蒋初蕾. 情绪启动对大学生工作记忆广度的影响[D]: [硕士学位论文]. 西安: 陕西师范大学, 2014. |
[6] | 白学军, 尹莎莎, 杨海波, 吕勇, 胡伟, 罗跃嘉. 视觉工作记忆内容对自下而上注意控制的影响: 一项ERP研究[J]. 心理学报, 2011, 43(10): 1103-1113. |
[7] | 杨昭宁, 顾子贝, 王杜娟, 谭旭运, 王晓明. 愤怒和悲伤情绪对助人决策的影响: 人际责任归因的作用[J]. 心理学报, 2017, 49(3): 393-403. |
[8] | Baddeley, A.D. (2000) The Episodic Buffer: A New Component of Working Memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 |
[9] | 毛伟宾, 杨治良. 工作记忆容量研究新进展[J]. 心理科学, 2008, 31(3): 741-743. |
[10] | 孙慧明, 傅小兰. 视觉工作记忆巩固机制: 固化抑或衰退[J]. 心理科学进展, 2011, 19(11): 1605-1614. |
[11] | 刘忆星, 周曙, 刘中华, 辛汤欣, 吴燕, 陈璇. 视觉工作记忆容量个体差异的事件相关电位时空模式研究[J]. 暨南大学学报(自然科学与医学版), 2013, 34(6): 604-609. |
[12] | 单西娇, 李寿欣. 由两个模型看视觉工作记忆容量机制的研究[J]. 心理科学进展, 2010, 18(11): 1684-1691. |
[13] | 殷莉. 不同人格类型大学生情绪状态对记忆影响的实验研究[D]: [硕士学位论文]. 广州: 华南师范大学, 2004. |
[14] | 段宁. 不同情绪启动状态下的注意稳定性对记忆广度的影响[J]. 太原师范学院学报(社会科学版), 2013, 12(3): 127-130. |
[15] | Lavric, A., Rippon, G. and Gray, J.R. (2003) Threat-Evoked Anxiety Disrupts Spatial Working Memory Performance: An Attentional Account. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 27, 489-504. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026300619569 |
[16] | 梁凤华, 曹立人. 视觉工作记忆研究进展[J]. 人类工效学, 2004, 10(2): 23-25. |