The use of superstitions, in healthcare settings, is often
used to understand uncontrollable events. In order to describe the knowledge
and clinical relevance of superstitions, survey data were collected from
nursing staff who provide direct patient care. Using a research-based,
study-specific survey, the aim of this study was to describe the knowledge and
clinical relevance of superstitions among
nurses who provide direct patient care in a hospital setting. These data
were analyzed to describe the study population, along with their knowledge and
clinical relevance of superstitions, and compare the responses based on
self-disclosure of superstitiousness. Participants that described themselves as
superstitious reported a greater propensity for superstitions to be clinically
relevant. Less education and younger age increased one’s use of superstitions
in a clinical setting. The use of superstitions provides a coping mechanism for
healthcare providers and a format to explain and understand uncontrollable
events.
References
[1]
Abredari, H., Bolourchifard. F., Rassouli. M., Nasiri, N., Taher, M., & Abedi A. (2015). Health Locus of Control and Self-Care Behaviors in Diabetic Foot Patients. Medical Journal of the Islam Republic of Iran, 29, 283-287.
[2]
Buhrmann, H. G., & Zuagg, M. (1981). Superstitions among Basketball Players an Investigation of Various Forms of Superstitious Belief and Behavior among Competitive Basketballers at the Junior High School to University Level. Journal of Sports Behavior, 4, 163-175.
[3]
Carlson, B. D., Mowen, J. C., & Fang, X. (2009). Trait Superstition and Consumer Behavior: Re-Conceptualization, Measurement, and Initial Investigations. Psychology & Marketing, 26, 689-713. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.20295
[4]
Chukkali, S., & Dey, A. M. (2020). Development and Validation of Superstitious Beliefs Scale. Rupkatha Journal on Interdisciplinary Studies in Humanities, 12, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.21659/rupkatha.v12n1.32
[5]
Faiza, A. (2018). Social and Psychological Factors for Superstition: A Brief Literature Review. International Journal of Advanced Study and Research Work, 1, 1-6.
[6]
Griffiths, O., Shehabi, N., Murphy, R. A., & LePelly, M. E. (2019). Superstition Predicts Perception of Illusory Control. British Journal of Psychology, 110, 499-518. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12344
[7]
Hirshleifer, D., Jian, M., & Zhang, H. (2018). Superstition and Financial Decision-Making. Manage Science, 64, 232-252. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.2016.2584
[8]
Hong, S. J. (2018). Gendered Cultural Identities: The Influences of Family and Privacy Boundaries, Subjective Norms, and Stigma Beliefs on Family Health History Communication. Health Communication, 33, 927-938. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1322480
[9]
Huque, M., & Huq Chowdhurry A. (2007). A Scale to Measure Superstition. Journal of Social Science, 3, 18-23.
[10]
Kashdan, T. B., & Rottenberg, J. (2010). Psychological Flexibility as a Fundamental Aspect of Health. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 865-878. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.001
[11]
Kuhl, D. (2003). What Dying People Want: Practical Wisdom for the End of Life. Public Affairs.
[12]
Matute, H., Yarritu, I., & Vadillo, M. A. (2011). Illusions of Causality at the Heart of Pseudoscience. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 392-405. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X532210
[13]
Neil, G., Anderson, B., & Sheppard, W. (1981). Superstitions among Male and Female Athletes at Various Levels of Involvement. Journal of Sport Behavior, 4, 137-148.
[14]
Omeje, O., & Nebo, C. (2011). The Influence of Locus Control on Adherence to Treatment Regimen among Hypertensive Patients. Patient Prefer Adherence, 5, 141-148. https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S15098
[15]
Parnell, J. A., Dent, E. B., O’Regan, N., & Hughes, T. (2012). Managing Performance in a Volatile Environment: Contrasting Perspectives on Luck and Causality. British Journal of Management, 23, S104-S118. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8551.2012.00815.x
[16]
Peterson, C. (1978). Locus of Control and Belief in Self-Oriented Superstitions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 105, 305-306. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1978.9924131
[17]
Phillips, D., & Clancy, K. (1972). Some Effects of “Social Desirability” in Survey Studies. American Journal of Sociology, 77, 921-940. https://doi.org/10.1086/225231
[18]
Ramezani, K., Ramezanu, M., & Ramezani, Z. (2016). Comparison of the Trend of Superstition in Educated Academics and Commons Kohgiluyeh and Boyer-Admad. International Journal of Humanities and Cultural Studies, 3, 496-510.
[19]
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. Macmillan.
[20]
Stapelberg, M. (2014). Strange but True. Crux Publishers.
[21]
Taher, M., Pashaeypoor, S., Cheraghi, M. A., Karimy, M., & Hoseini, A. S. S. (2020). Superstition in Health Beliefs: Concept Exploration and Development. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care, 9, 1325-1330. https://doi.org/10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_871_19
[22]
Taher, M., Safavi Bayat, Z., Ghasemi, E., Abredari, H., Karimy, M., & Abedi, A. R. (2015). Correlation between Compliance Regimens with Health Locus of Control in Patients with Hypertension. Medical Journal of Islam Republic of Iran, 29, 194-196.
[23]
Torgler, B. (2007). Determinants of Superstition. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 36, 713-733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2007.01.007
[24]
Vyse, S.A. (1997). Believing in Magic: The Psychology of Superstition. Oxford University Press.
[25]
Whitson, J. A., & Galinsky, J. D. (2008). Lacking Control Increases Illusionary Pattern Perception. Science, 322, 115-117. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1159845
[26]
Wong, S. H. (2012). Does Superstition help? A Study of the Role of Superstitions and Death Beliefs on Death Anxiety Amongst Chinese Undergraduates in Hong Kong. Omega, 65, 55-70. https://doi.org/10.2190/OM.65.1.d