全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

主语熟悉度对否定结构理解的影响
The Influence of Subject Familiarity on the Interpretation of Negative Constructions

DOI: 10.12677/ML.2021.92054, PP. 390-396

Keywords: 反语理解,主语熟悉度,否定结构
Irony Interpretation
, Subject Familiarity, Negative Construction

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

通过以“X不是最……的”的中文否定结构作为实验材料,研究主语熟悉度对话语理解所产生的影响,并进一步考察现代反语加工理论模型的中文适用性。结果显示,在主语熟悉度高的否定句子中,人们会自动将其解释为讽刺的;而主语熟悉度相对较低的否定句会被评判为字面义的。主语熟悉度会影响个体的主观判断,应在实验中被严格控制。
This paper studies the influence of subject familiarity on the interpretation of Chinese negative structure in the form of “X is not the most …”, and further investigates the applicability of contemporary irony processing models to Chinese. The results show that negative constructions with high subject familiarity are interpreted and rate as ironic by default, while negative sentences with relatively low subject familiarity are judged as literal. Subject familiarity could affect individual's subjective judgment and should be strictly controlled in interpretation experiment.

References

[1]  姚俊. 论反语理解的认知机制及其语用效力[J]. 四川外语学院学报, 2003, 19(5): 95-98.
[2]  张萌. 反语认知的心理学研究[M]. 广州: 暨南大学出版社, 2010: 47-58.
[3]  Colston, H.L. (2000) Contrast and Pragmatics in Figurative Language: Anything Understatement Can Do, Irony Can Do Better. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1557-1583.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00110-1
[4]  Jorgensen, J. (1996) The Function of Sarcastic Irony in Speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 26, 613-634.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(95)00067-4
[5]  刘正光. 反语理论综述[J]. 解放军外国语学院学报, 2002, 25(4): 16-20.
[6]  Grice, P.H. (1975) Logic and Conversation. In: Cole, P. and Morgans, J., Eds., Speech Acts: Syntax and Semantics, Vol. 3, Academic, New York, 41-58.
[7]  Searle, J.R. (1979) Expression and Meaning: Studies in the Theory of Speech Acts. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511609213
[8]  Wilson, D. and Sperber, D. (1992) On Verbal Irony. Lingua, 87, 53-76.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(92)90025-E
[9]  Giora, R. (1997) Understanding Figurative and Literal Language: The Graded Salience Hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 183-206.
https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183
[10]  Giora, R. (2003) On Our Mind: Salience, Context, and Figurative Language. Oxford University, New York, 13-39.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195136166.003.0002
[11]  Giora, R., Livnat, E., Fein, O., Barnea, A., Zeiman, R. and Berger, I. (2013) Negation Generates Nonliteral Interpretations by Default. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 89-115.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2013.768510
[12]  黄彬瑶. 语境制约汉语反语认知加工的神经心理机制研究[D]: [博士学位论文]. 杭州: 浙江大学, 2015.
[13]  Giora, R. and Fein, O. (1999) On Understanding Familiar and Less-Familiar Figurative Language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 1601-1618.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00006-5
[14]  宫同喜. R. Giora的“等级凸显假设”观点介绍[J]. 当代语言学, 2009, 11(3): 275-278.
[15]  Giora, R. (1998) On Understanding Figurative and Literal Language: The Graded Salience Hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 183-206.
https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183
[16]  Pexman, P., Ferretti, T. and Katz, A.N. (2000) Discourse Factors That Influence Online Reading of Metaphor and Irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 201-222.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950dp2903_2
[17]  Schwoebel, J., Dews, S., Winner, E. and Srinivas, K. (2000) Obligatory Processing of the Literal Meaning of Ironic Utterances: Further Evidence. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 47-61.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2000.9678864
[18]  Ivanko, L.S. and Pexman, M.P. (2003) Context Incongruity and Irony Processing. Discourse Processes, 35, 241-279.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3503_2
[19]  Giora, R, Fein, O., Laadan, D. Wolfson, J., Zeituny, M., Kidron, R., Kaufman, R. and Shaham, R. (2007) Expecting Irony: Context versus Salience-Based Effects. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 119-146.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926480701235346
[20]  Giora, R., Fein, O. and Mendelson, I. (2015) Default Sarcastic Interpretations: On the Priority of Nonsalient Interpretations. Discourse Processes, 52, 173-200.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.954951
[21]  Giora, R., Givoni, S. and Fein, O. (2015) Defaultness Reigns: The Case of Sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 30, 290-313.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2015.1074804
[22]  Fein, O., Yeari, M. and Giora, R. (2015) On the Priority of Salience-Based Interpretations: The Case of Sarcastic Irony. Intercultural Pragmatics, 12, 1-32.
https://doi.org/10.1515/ip-2015-0001
[23]  Becker, I. and Giora, R. (2018) The Defaultness Hypothesis: A Quantitative Corpus-Based Study of Non/Default Sarcasm and Literalness Production. Journal of Pragmatics, 138, 149-164.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2018.09.013
[24]  Giora, R. and Becker, I. (2019) She Is Not the Most Sparkling Drink in the Pub Global vs. Local Cue—Which Reigns Supreme? Metaphor and Symbol, 34, 141-157.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2019.1649836
[25]  Giora, R., Cholev, A., Fein, O. and Peleg, O. (2018) On the Superiority of Defaultness: Hemispheric Perspectives of Processing Negative and Affirmative Sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 33, 163-174.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2018.1481259
[26]  Giora, R., Drucker, A. and Fein, O. (2014) Resonating with Default Nonsalient Interpretations: A Corpus-Based Study of Negative Sarcasm. Belgian Journal of Linguistics, 28, 3-18.
https://doi.org/10.1075/bjl.28.01gio

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133