|
使用橡胶手错觉及其变式研究身体表征
|
Abstract:
[1] | 曾向, 黄希庭(2001). 国外关于身体自我的研究. 心理学动态, 9(1), 41-46. |
[2] | 周爱保, 朱婧, 夏瑞雪, 李世峰, 徐科朋, 张荣华, 等(2013). 我观故我在?——从橡胶手错觉对自我身体所有权的探讨. 心理科学, 36(6), 1328-1332. |
[3] | Armel, K. C., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2003). Projecting Sensations to External Objects: Evidence from Skin Conductance Response. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270, 1499-1506.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2364 |
[4] | Botvinick, M. (2004). Probing the Neural Basis of Body Ownership. Science, 305, 782-783.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1101836 |
[5] | Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber Hands “Feel” Touch That Eyes See. Nature, 391, 756-756.
https://doi.org/10.1038/35784 |
[6] | Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). The Rubber Hand Illusion: Sensitivity and Reference Frame for Body Ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 229-240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.001 |
[7] | D’Alonzo, M., & Cipriani, C. (2012). Vibrotactile Sensory Substitution Elicits Feeling of Ownership of an Alien Hand. PLoS ONE, 7, e50756. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050756 |
[8] | De Preester, H., & Tsakiris, M. (2009). Body-Extension versus Body-Incorporation: Is There a Need for a Body-Model? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307-319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-009-9121-y |
[9] | De Vignemont, F. (2010). Widening the Body to Rubber Hands and Tools: What’s the Difference? Revue de Neuropsychologie, Neurosciences Cognitives et Cliniques, 2, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3917/rne.023.0203 |
[10] | Dummer, T., Picot-Annand, A., Neal, T., & Moore, C. (2009). Move-ment and the Rubber Hand Illusion. Perception, 38, 271-280. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5921 |
[11] | Ehrsson, H. H., Rosén, B., Stockselius, A., Ragn?, C., K?hler, P., & Lundborg, G. (2008). Upper Limb Amputees Can Be Induced to Experience a Rubber Hand as Their Own. Brain, 131, 3443-3452. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn297 |
[12] | Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). That’s My Hand! Activity in Premotor Cortex Reflects Feeling of Ownership of a Limb. Science, 305, 875-877. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1097011 |
[13] | Eshkevari, E., Rieger, E., Longo, M. R., Haggard, P., & Treasure, J. (2012). Increased Plasticity of the Bodily Self in Eating Disorders. Psychological Medicine, 42, 819-828. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002091 |
[14] | Farmer, H., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., & Tsakiris, M. (2012). Beyond the Colour of My Skin: How Skin Colour Affects the Sense of Body-Ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1242-1256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.011 |
[15] | Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical Conceptions of the Self: Implications for Cognitive Science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 14-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5 |
[16] | Gallagher, S. (2005). How the Body Shapes the Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/0199271941.001.0001 |
[17] | Gillihan, S. J., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Is Self Special? A Critical Review of Evidence from Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 76-97. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.76 |
[18] | Graziano, M. S., & Botvinick, M. M. (2002). How the Brain Represents the Body: Insights from Neurophysiology and Psychology. In Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance XIX (pp. 136-157). Oxford: Oxford University Press. |
[19] | Graziano, M. S., Cooke, D. F., & Taylor, C. S. (2000). Coding the Location of the Arm by Sight. Science, 290, 1782-1786.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.290.5497.1782 |
[20] | Haggard, P., & Jundi, S. (2009). Rubber Hand Illusions and Size-Weight Illusions: Self-Representation Modulates Representation of External Objects. Perception, 38, 1796-1803. https://doi.org/10.1068/p6399 |
[21] | H?gni, K., Eng, K., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., Holper, L., Keisker, B., Siekierka, E., & Kiper, D. C. (2008). Observing Virtual arms That You Imagine Are Yours Increases the Galvanic Skin Response to an Unexpected Threat. PLoS ONE, 3, e3082.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003082 |
[22] | Hohwy, J., & Paton, B. (2010). Explaining Away the Body: Experiences of Supernaturally Caused Touch and Touch on Non- Hand Objects within the Rubber Hand Illusion. PLoS ONE, 5, e9416. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0009416 |
[23] | Ide, M. (2013). The Effect of “Anatomical Plausibility” of Hand Angle on the Rubber Hand Illusion. Perception, 42, 103-111.
https://doi.org/10.1068/p7322 |
[24] | Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. (2012). Moving a Rubber Hand That Feels Like Your Own: A Dissociation of Ownership and Agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040 |
[25] | Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014a). The Spatial Distance Rule in the Moving and Classical Rubber Hand Illusions. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 118-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.022 |
[26] | Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014b). The Moving Rubber Hand Illusion Revisited: Comparing Movements and Visuotactile Stimulation to Induce Illusory Ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 26, 117-132.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003 |
[27] | Kokkinara, E., & Slater, M. (2014). Measuring the Effects through Time of the Influence of Visuomotor and Visuotactile Synchronous Stimulation on a Virtual Body Ownership Illusion. Perception, 43, 43-58. https://doi.org/10.1068/p7545 |
[28] | Liepelt, R., Dolk, T., & Hommel, B. (2017). Self-Perception beyond the Body: The Role of Past Agency. Psychological Research, 81, 549-559. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0766-1 |
[29] | Lloyd, D. M. (2007). Spatial Limits on Referred Touch to an Alien Limb May Reflect Boundaries of Visuo-Tactile Peripersonal Space Surrounding the Hand. Brain and Cognition, 64, 104-109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2006.09.013 |
[30] | Longo, M. R. (2015). Implicit and Explicit Body Representations. European Psychologist, 20, 6-15.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000198 |
[31] | Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M. P., Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2009). Self Awareness and the Body Image. Acta Psychologica, 132, 166-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.02.003 |
[32] | Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2013). The Virtual-Hand Illusion: Effects of Impact and Threat on Perceived Ownership and Affective Resonance. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 604. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00604 |
[33] | Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2015a). Body-Ownership for Actively Operated Non-Corporeal Objects. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 75-86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.003 |
[34] | Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2015b). The Role of Agency for Per-ceived Ownership in the Virtual Hand Illusion. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 277-288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.008 |
[35] | Ma, K., Hommel, B., & Chen, H. (2018). The Roles of Consistency and Exclusivity in Perceiving Body Ownership and Agency. Psychological Research, 83, 175-184. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-0978-7 |
[36] | Maister, L., Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Tsakiris, M. (2013). Experiencing Ownership over a Dark-Skinned Body Reduces Implicit Racial Bias. Cognition, 128, 170-178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.002 |
[37] | Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). On the Other Hand: Dummy Hands and Peripersonal Space. Behavioural Brain Research, 191, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.041 |
[38] | Maravita, A., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2003). Multisensory Integration and the Body Schema: Close to Hand and within Reach. Current Biology, 13, R531-R539. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00449-4 |
[39] | Moseley, G. L., Olthof, N., Venema, A., Don, S., Wijers, M., Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2008). Psychologically Induced Cooling of a Specific Body Part Caused by the Illusory Ownership of an Artificial Counterpart. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 13169-13173. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0803768105 |
[40] | Pavani, F., & Zampini, M. (2007). The Role of Hand Size in the Fake-Hand Illusion Paradigm. Perception, 36, 1547.
https://doi.org/10.1068/p5853 |
[41] | Perez-Marcos, D., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Slater, M. (2012). Is My Hand Connected to My Body? The Impact of Body Continuity and Arm Alignment on the Virtual Hand Illusion. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 6, 295-305.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11571-011-9178-5 |
[42] | Preston, C. (2013). The Role of Distance from the Body and Distance from the Real Hand in Ownership and Disownership during the Rubber Hand Illusion. Acta Psychologica, 142, 177-183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.12.005 |
[43] | Sanchez-Vives, M. V., Spanlang, B., Frisoli, A., Bergamasco, M., & Slater, M. (2010). Virtual Hand Illusion Induced by Visuomotor Correlations. PLoS ONE, 5, e10381. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010381 |
[44] | Schütz-Bosbach, S., Tausche, P., & Weiss, C. (2009). Roughness Perception during the Rubber Hand Illusion. Brain and Cognition, 70, 136-144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.01.006 |
[45] | Seiryte, A., & Rusconi, E. (2015). The Empathy Quotient (EQ) Predicts Perceived Strength of Bodily Illusions and Illusion-Related Sensations of Pain. Personality & Individual Differences, 77, 112-117.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.048 |
[46] | Short, F., & Ward, R. (2009). Virtual Limbs and Body Space: Critical Features for the Distinction between Body Space and Near-Body Space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1092-1103.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015873 |
[47] | Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2008). Towards a Digital Body: The Virtual Arm Illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.09.006.2008 |
[48] | Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing Illusory Ownership of a Virtual Body. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 2, 214-220. https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.01.029.2009 |
[49] | Tsakiris, M. (2010). My Body in the Brain: A Neurocognitive Model of Body-Ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48, 703-712.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034 |
[50] | Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2005). The Rubber Hand Illusion Revisited: Visuotactile Integration and Self-Attribution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 80-91.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.80 |
[51] | Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G., & Haggard, P. (2006). Having a Body versus Moving Your Body: How Agency Structures Body- Ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 423-432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.004 |
[52] | Tsakiris, M., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Gallagher, S. (2007). On Agency and Body-Ownership: Phenomenological and Neurocognitive Reflections. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 645-660. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.05.012 |
[53] | Zhang, J., Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2015). The Virtual Hand Illusion Is Moderated by Context-Induced Spatial Reference Frames. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1659. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01659 |
[54] | Zopf, R., Savage, G., & Williams, M. A. (2010). Cross-Modal Congruency Measures of Lateral Distance Effects on the Rubber Hand Illusion. Neuropsychologia, 48, 713-725. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.028 |