|
口语反讽认知要素——语境和音韵
|
Abstract:
[1] | 黄彬瑶(2015). 语境制约汉语反语认知加工的神经心理机制研究. 博士论文, 杭州: 浙江大学. |
[2] | 章婷, 顾文涛(2011). 汉语情感语音的语料设计与韵律研究. 南京师范大学文学院学报, 3, 33-44. |
[3] | Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Infantino, M. G. (2000). Irony as a Game of Implicitness: Acoustic Pro?les of Ironic Communication. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 275-311. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005100221723 |
[4] | Colston, H. L., & O’Brien, J. (2000). Contrast and Pragmatics in Figurative Language: Anything Understatement Can Do, Irony Can Do Better. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 1557-1583. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00110-1 |
[5] | Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., & Kissine, M. (2017). Perspective-Taking and Frugal Strategies: Evidence from Sarcasm Detection. Journal of Pragmatics, 119, 33-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2017.08.002 |
[6] | Gibbs, R. W. (1986). On the Psycholinguistics of Sarcasm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 3-15.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.115.1.3 |
[7] | Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The Poetics of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
[8] | Giora, R. (1997). Understanding Figurative and Literal Language: The Graded Salience Hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 183-206. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.1997.8.3.183 |
[9] | Giora, R. (1999). On the Priority of Salient Meanings: Studies of Literal and Figurative Language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 919-929. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00100-3 |
[10] | Giora, R. (2003). On Our Mind: Salience, Context and Figurative Language. New York: Oxford University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195136166.001.0001 |
[11] | Grice, P. H. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics: Speech Acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004368811_003 |
[12] | Jeffrey, H. T., Philip, D. J., & Kelly, P. (2000). Children’s Comprehension of Critical and Complimentary Forms of Verbal Irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 227-248. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD010204 |
[13] | Kreuz, R. J., & Glucksberg, S. (1989). How to Be Sarcastic: The Echoic Reminder Theory of Verbal Irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 374-386. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.4.374 |
[14] | Kumon-Nakanura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about Another Piece of Pie: The Allusional Pretense Theory of Discourse Irony? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 3-21.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.1.3 |
[15] | Mehrabian, A., & Ferris, S. R. (1967). Inference of Attitudes from Nonverbal Communication in Two Channels. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 248-252. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024648 |
[16] | Pell, M. D., Paulmann, S., Dara, C., Alasseri, A., & Kotz, S. A. (2009). Factors in the Recognition of Vocally Expressed Emotions: A Comparison of Four Languages. Journal of Phonetics, 7, 417-435.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2009.07.005 |
[17] | Rivière, E., Klein, M., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2018). Using Context and Prosody in Irony Understanding: Variability amongst Individuals. Journal of Pragmatics, 138, 165-172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2018.10.006 |
[18] | Searle, J. R. (1979). Expression and Meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
[19] | Shapley, M. (1987). Prosodic Variation and Audience Response. Papers in Pragmatics, 1, 66-79.
https://doi.org/10.1075/iprapip.1.2.03sha |
[20] | Voyer, D., Thibodeau, S. H., & Delong, B. J. (2014). Context, Contrast, and Tone of Voice in Auditory Sarcasm Perception. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 29-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9323-5 |
[21] | Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2012). Meaning and Relevance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139028370 |
[22] | Woodland, J., & Voyer, D. (2011). Context and Intonation in the Perception of Sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 26, 227-239.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2011.583197 |