|
Modern Linguistics 2020
多词素词识别中词素分解与TL效应
|
Abstract:
[1] | Diependaele, K., Du?abeitia, J.A., Morris, J. and Keuleers, E. (2011) Fast Morphological Effects in First and Second Language Word Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 344-358.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.01.003 |
[2] | Du?abeitia, J.A., Perea, M. and Carreiras, M. (2007) Do Trans-posed-Letter Similarity Effects Occur at a Morpheme Level? Evidence for Morpho-Orthographic Decomposition. Cognition, 105, 691-703.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.001 |
[3] | Longtin, C.M. and Meunier, F. (2005) Morphological De-composition in Early Visual Word Processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 26-41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.008 |
[4] | Rastle, K., Davis, M.H. and New, B. (2004) The Broth in My Brother’s Brothel: Morpho-Orthographic Segmentation in Visual Word Recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 1090-1098. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196742 |
[5] | Taft, M. (2003) Morphological Representation as a Correlation between Form and Meaning. In: Reading Complex Words, Neuropschology and Cognition, Volume 22, Springer US, Berlin, 113-137.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-3720-2_6 |
[6] | Meunier, F. and Longtin, C.M. (2007) Morphological De-composition and Semantic Integration in Word Processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 457-471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.005 |
[7] | Giraudo, H. and Grainger, J. (2000) Effects of Prime Word Fre-quency and Cumulative Root Frequency in Masked Morphological Priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 421-444. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960050119652 |
[8] | McClelland, J.L. and Rumelhart, D.E. (1981) An In-teractive Activation Model of Context Effects in Letter Perception: Part I. An Account of Basic Findings. Psychologi-cal Review, 88, 375-407. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375 |
[9] | Christianson, K., Johnson, R.L. and Rayner, K. (2005) Letter Transpositions within and across Morphemes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1327-1339.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1327 |
[10] | Acha, J. and Perea, M. (2008) The Effect of Neighborhood Frequency in Reading: Evidence with Transposed-Letter Neighbors. Cognition, 108, 290-300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.006 |
[11] | Andrews, S. (1996) Lexical Retrieval and Selection Processes: Effects of Transposed-Letter Confusability. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 775-800. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0040 |
[12] | Perea, M., Rosa, E. and Gómez, C. (2005) The Frequency Effect for Pseudowords in the Lexical Decision Task. Perception and Psychophysics, 67, 301-314. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206493 |
[13] | Perea, M. and Lupker, S.J. (2003) Transposed-Letter Confusability Ef-fects in Masked form Priming. In: Masked Priming: State of the Art, Psychology Press, Hove, 97-120. |
[14] | Perea, M. and Lupker, S.J. (2004) Can CANISO Activate CASINO? Transposed-Letter Similarity Effects with Nonadjacent Let-ter Positions. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 231-246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.05.005 |
[15] | Schoonbaert, S. and Grainger, J. (2004) Letter Position Coding in Printed Word Perception: Effects of Repeated and Transposed Letters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 333-367. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000198 |
[16] | Frankish, C. and Turner, E. (2007) SIHGT and SUNOD: The Role of Orthography and Phonology in the Perception of Transposed Letter Anagrams. Journal of Mem-ory and Language, 56, 189-211.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.002 |
[17] | Lee, C.H. and Taft, M. (2011) Subsyllabic Structure Reflected in Letter Confusability Effects in Korean Word Recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 129-134. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0028-y |
[18] | Perea, M. and Carreiras, M. (2006) Do Transposed-Letter Simi-larity Effects Occur at a Prelexical Phonological Level? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1600-1613. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210500298880 |
[19] | Rastle, K. and Davis, M.H. (2008) Morphological Decomposi-tion Based on the Analysis of Orthography. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 942-971. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802069730 |
[20] | Taft, M. (2004) Morphological Decomposition and the Reverse Base Frequency Effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 745-765. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980343000477 |
[21] | Rueckl, J.G. and Rimzhim, A. (2011) On the Interaction of Letter Transpositions and Morphemic Boundaries. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 482-508. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.500020 |
[22] | Masserang, K.M. and Pollatsek, A. (2012) Transposed Letter Effects in Prefixed Words: Implications for Morphological Decomposition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 476-495. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.658037 |
[23] | Sánchez-Gutiérrez, C. and Rastle, K. (2013) Letter Transpositions within and across Morphemic Boundaries: Is There a Cross-Language Difference? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 988-996.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0425-0 |
[24] | Beyersmann, E., McCormick, S.F. and Rastle, K. (2013) Letter Transpositions within Morphemes and across Morpheme Boundaries. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2389-2410.
https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.782326 |
[25] | Norris, D. and Kinoshita, S. (2008) Perception as Evidence Accumulation and Bayesian Inference: Insights from Masked Priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 433-455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012799 |
[26] | Kinoshita, S. and Norris, D. (2009) Transposed-Letter Priming of Orthographic Representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 35, 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014277 |
[27] | Du?abeitia, J.A., Kinoshita, S., Carreiras, M. and Norris, D. (2011) Is Morpho-Orthographic Decomposition Purely Orthographic? Evidence from Masked Priming in the Same-Different Task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 509-529. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.499215 |
[28] | Beyersmann, E., Castles, A. and Coltheart, M. (2011) Early Morphological Decomposition during Visual Word Recognition: Evidence from Masked Transposed-Letter Priming. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 937-942.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0120-y |
[29] | Beyersmann, E., Du?abeitia, J.A., Carreiras, M., Coltheart, M. and Castles, A. (2013) Early Morphological Decomposition of Suf?xed Words: Masked Priming Evidence with Transposed-Letter Nonword Primes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 869-892. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000057 |
[30] | Taft, M. and Nillsen, C. (2013) Morphological Decomposition and the Transposed-Letter (TL) Position Effect. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 917-938. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.679662 |
[31] | Leck, K.J., Weekes, B.S. and Chen, M.J. (1995) Visual and Phonological Pathways to the Lexicon: Evidence from Chinese Readers. Memory and Cognition, 23, 468-476. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197248 |
[32] | Ding, G., Peng, D. and Taft, M. (2004) The Nature of the Mental Re-presentation of Radicals in Chinese: A Priming Study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 530.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.530 |
[33] | 丁国盛, 彭聃龄. 汉语逆序词识别中整词与词素的关系[J]. 当代语言学, 2006(8): 36-45. |
[34] | Bai, C., Cai, S. and Schumacher, P.B. (2011) Reversibility in Chinese Word For-mation Influences Target Identification. Neuroscience Letters, 499, 14-18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.020 |
[35] | Zhou, X. and Marslen-Wilson, W. (1999) The Nature of Sub-lexical Processing in Reading Chinese Characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog-nition, 25, 819-837.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.4.819 |
[36] | Orfanidou, E., Davis, M.H. and Marslen-Wilson, W.D. (2011) Orthographic and Semantic Opacity in Masked and Delayed Priming: Evidence from Greek. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 530-557.
https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.509055 |