全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

音乐训练对复合字母刺激心理旋转的影响
The Influence of Music Training on Mental Rotation of Compound Letter Stimuli

DOI: 10.12677/AP.2019.96130, PP. 1057-1063

Keywords: 心理旋转,整体优先效应,复合字母,音乐训练
Mental Rotation
, Global Precedence, Compound Letter, Music Training

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

本实验探讨音乐训练对复合字母心理旋转任务的影响。采用复合字母刺激探讨经历长期音乐训练的被试与没训练过的被试完成心理旋转任务的绩效是否存在差异。结果发现,音乐组被试在大字母旋转和小字母旋转任务中的正确率不存在显著差异,而非音乐训练组在大字母旋转任务中的正确率显著高于小字母旋转任务,表现出整体优先效应。音乐训练削弱了心理旋转任务中的整体优先效应。
The present study investigated the effect of music training on mental rotation of compound stimuli. Compound letters were used to explore whether the performance in the mental rotation task was different between musicians and non-musicians. The accuracy of global rotation task was not significantly different from local rotation task for musicians. However, the accuracy of global rotation task was greater than local rotation task for non-musicians, showing an overall priority effect. Music training weakened the overall priority effect of the mental rotation task.

References

[1]  邱香, 傅小兰, 隋丹妮, 李健, 唐一源(2009). 复合字母刺激心理旋转加工中的整体优先效应. 心理学报, 41(1), 1-9.
[2]  Bethell-Fox, C. E., & Shepard, R. N. (1988). Mental Rotation: Effects of Stimulus Complexity and Familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 14, 12-23.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.12
[3]  Bodner, M., Muftuler, L. T., Nalcioglu, O. et al. (2013). fMRI Study Relevant to the Mozart Effect: Brain Areas Involved in Spatial-Temporal Reasoning. Neurological Research, 23, 683-690.
https://doi.org/10.1179/016164101101199108
[4]  Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric Studies of the Rotation of Mental Images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual Information Processing (pp. 75-176). New York: Academic Press.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-170150-5.50009-3
[5]  Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain Structures Differ between Musicians and Non-Musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 9240-9245.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003
[6]  Hetland, L. (2000). Learning to Make Music Enhances Spatial Reasoning. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 179-238.
https://doi.org/10.2307/3333643
[7]  Jansen-Osmann, P., & Heil, M. (2007). Suitable Stimuli to Obtain (No) Gender Differences in the Speed of Cognitive Processes Involved in Mental Rotation. Brain & Cognition, 64, 217-227.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.002
[8]  McGee, M. G. (1979). Human Spatial Abilities: Psychometric Studies and Environmental, Genetic, Hormonal, and Neurological Influences. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 889-918.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.5.889
[9]  Navon, D. (1977). Forest before Trees: The Precedence of Global Features in Visual Perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3
[10]  Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Asada, T. et al. (2001). Functional Anatomy of Musical Perception in Musicians. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 754-760.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/11.8.754
[11]  Ouimet, T., Foster, N. E., & Hyde, K. L. (2012). Auditory Global-Local Processing: Effects of Attention and Musical Experience. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, 2536-2544.
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4747009
[12]  Pietsch, S., & Jansen, P. (2012). Different Mental Rotation Performance in Students of Music, Sport and Education. Learning & Individual Differences, 22, 159-163.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.012
[13]  Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., & Ky, C. N. (1993). Music and Spatial Task Performance. Nature, 365, 611.
https://doi.org/10.1038/365611a0
[14]  Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L. et al. (2006). Spatial Representation of Pitch Height: The SMARC Effect. Cognition, 99, 113-129.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.004
[15]  Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). Music and Cognitive Abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 317-320.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00389.x
[16]  Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Readings in Cognitive Science, 171, 701-703.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701
[17]  Sluming, V., Brooks, J., Howard, M. et al. (2007). Broca’s Area Supports Enhanced Visuospatial Cognition in Orchestral Musicians. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 27, 3799-3806.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0147-07.2007
[18]  Stoesz, B. M., Jakobson, L. S., Kilgour, A. R. et al. (2007). Local Processing Advantage in Musicians: Evidence from Disembedding and Constructional Tasks. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 153-165.
https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2007.25.2.153
[19]  Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 47, 599-604.
https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599
[20]  Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: A Meta-Analysis and Consideration of Critical Variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250-270.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.250
[21]  White, E. J., Hutka, S. A., Williams, L. J. et al. (2013). Learning, Neural Plasticity and Sensitive Periods: Implications for Language Acquisition, Music Training and Transfer across the Lifespan. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7, 90.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2013.00090

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133