Spatial neglect syndrome with cerebral lesions is characterized by the ina-bility to orient, report, or respond to relevant visual stimuli contralateral to the lesions. In this study, we investigated the pattern of horizontal visual search in a large space; this search was performed by patients with right hemisphere damage caused by cerebrovascular disease. The neck rotation angle and search time in each increment were continuously recorded during the task, and quantitative data of the measurements were collected. Head position during the visual search task in a large space (%) was then calculated. We set angular bands in increments of 5 degrees from the midline on the left and right sides, and calculated the total search time in each angular band. In patients with unilateral spatial neglect (USN), the search time in the angular band of 5 - 9 on the right side was significantly longer (with USN 16.7%, without USN 4.5%, t = 2.52, df = 16, p < 0.05). Furthermore, in patients with neglect in the leftmost end area in a large space, the search time was significantly shorter in the angular band of 1 - 4 degrees to the left (with neglect 13.1%, without neglect 23.7%, t = 2.13, df = 16, p < 0.05), and tended to be long in the angular band of 5 - 9 to the right. The neck rotation angle and pattern during the search task in a large field deviated slightly to the right, and the search time slightly to the left was short. We believe that these results support the rightward deviation of the search pattern and frame of USN patients.
References
[1]
Heilman, K.M., Watson, R.T. and Valenstein, E. (1985) Neglect and Related Disorders. In: Heilman, K.M., et al., Eds., Clinical Neuropsychology, 2nd Edition, Oxford University Press, New York, 243-293.
Shelton, P.A., Bowers, D. and Heilman, K.M. (1990) Peripersonal and Vertical Neglect. Brain, 113, 191-205. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/113.1.191
[4]
Halligan, P.W. and Marshall, J.C. (1991) Left Neglect for Near but Not Far Space in Man. Nature, 350, 498-500. https://doi.org/10.1038/350498a0
[5]
Mennemeier, M., Wertman, E. and Heilman, K.M. (1992) Neglect of Near Peripersonal Space. Evidence for Multidirectional Attentional Systems in Humans. Brain, 115, 37-50. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/115.1.37
[6]
Sunahara, N. and Notoya, M. (2005) Relationship between Sound Lateralization Abilities and Laterality Index by Behavioural Inattention Test in Unilateral Spatial Neglect Patients. Journal of the Tsuruma Health Science Society, 29, 11-19.
[7]
Nakatani, K., Notoya, M., Sunahara, N., et al. (2013) Horizontal Visual Search in a Large Field by Patients with Unilateral Spatial Neglect. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 20, 837-841. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2012.07.014
[8]
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E. and McHugh, P.R. (1975) Mini-Mental State. A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-898. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
[9]
Ishiai, S. (1999) Behavioural Inattention Test. Japanese Edition, Shinkoh-Igaku Publisher, Tokyo.
[10]
Ishiai, S., Koyama, Y., Seki, K., et al. (2000) Unilateral Spatial Neglect in AD: Significance of Line Bisection Performance. Neurology, 8, 364-370.
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.55.3.364
[11]
Rossetti, Y., Rode, G., Pisella, L., et al. (1998) Prism Adaptation to a Rightward Optical Deviation Rehabilitates Left Hemispatial Neglect. Nature, 395, 166-169.
https://doi.org/10.1038/25988
[12]
Frassinetti, F., Angeli, V., Meneghello, F., et al. (2002) Long-Lasting Amelioration of Visuospatial Neglect by Prism Adaptation. Brain, 125, 608-623.
https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf056
[13]
Karnath, H.O., Niemeier, M. and Dichgans, J. (1998) Space Exploration in Neglect. Brain, 121, 2357-2367. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.12.2357
[14]
Karnath, H.O. (1997) Spatial Orientation and the Representation of Space with Parietal Lobe Lesions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 352, 1411-1419. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1997.0127
[15]
Ferber, S. and Karnath, H.O. (1999) Parietal and Occipital Lobe Contributions to Perception of Straight Ahead Orientation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 67, 572-578. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.67.5.572
[16]
Karnath, H.O., Schenkel, P. and Fischer, B. (1991) Trunk Orientation as the Determining Factor of the Contralateral Deficit in the Neglect Syndrome and as the Physical Anchor of the Internal Representation of Body Orientation in Space. Brain, 114, 1997-2014. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.4.1997
[17]
Karnath, H.O., Christ, K. and Hartje, W. (1993) Decrease of Contralateral Neglect by Neck Muscle Vibration and Spatial Orientation of Trunk Midline. Brain, 116, 383-396. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/116.2.383
[18]
Karnath, H.O. (1995) Transcutaneous Electrical Stimulation and Vibration of Neck Muscles in Neglect. Experimental Brain Research, 105, 321-324.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240969
[19]
Schindler, I., Kerkhoff, G., Karnath, H.O., et al. (2002) Neck Muscle Vibration Induces Lasting Recovery in Spatial Neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 73, 412-419. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.73.4.412
[20]
Johannsen, L., Ackermann, H. and Karnath, H.O. (2003) Lasting Amelioration of Spatial Neglect by Treatment with Neck Muscle Vibration Even without Concurrent Training. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 35, 249-253.
https://doi.org/10.1080/16501970310009972