|
自我文化效应的评估方法述评
|
Abstract:
[1] | 蔡华俭(2003). 内隐自尊效应及内隐自尊与外显自尊的关系. 心理学报, 35, 796-801. |
[2] | 黄希庭, 尹天子(2012). 从自尊的文化差异说起. 心理科学, 35(1), 2-8. |
[3] | 张力, 周天罡, 张剑, 刘祖祥, 范津, 朱滢(2005). 寻找中国人的自我: 一项fMRI研究. 中国科学C辑, 35, 472-478. |
[4] | 朱滢, 张力(2001). 自我记忆效应的实验研究. 中国科学C辑, 31(6), 537-543. |
[5] | Bochner, S. (1994). Cross-Cultural Differences in the Self Concept. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psy-chology, 25, 273-283.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022194252007 |
[6] | Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is This “We”? Levels of Collective Identity and Self Representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 83-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83 |
[7] | Cai, H., Brown, J. D., Deng, C., & Oakes, M. A. (2007). Self-Esteem and Culture: Differences in Cognitive Self-Evaluations or Affective Self-Regard? Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 162-170.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2007.00222.x |
[8] | Cai, H., Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., Wang, C., Carvallo, M., Xu, Y. et al. (2011). Tactical Self-Enhancement in China: Is Modesty at the Service of Self-Enhancement in East Asian Culture? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 59- 64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550610376599 |
[9] | Chen, S., Chen, K. Y., & Shaw, L. (2004). Self-Verification Mo-tives at the Collective Level of Self-Definition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 77-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.77 |
[10] | Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano, Y., Saito, D. et al. (2009). Neural Basis of Individualistic and Collectivistic Views of Self. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 2813-2820. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20707 |
[11] | Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2001). Cultural Orientations in the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 348-364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032003006 |
[12] | D’Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Bal-teau, E., Luxen, A. et al. (2007). Distinct Regions of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Are Associated with Self-Referential Processing and Perspective Taking. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 935-944. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935 |
[13] | Hamamura, T. (2011). Are Cultures Becoming Individualistic? A Cross-Temporal Comparison of Individualism-Collectiv- ism in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psy-chology Review, 36, 3-24. |
[14] | Harada, T., Li, Z., & Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Differential Dorsal and Ventral Medial Prefrontal Representations of the Implicit Self Modulated by Individualism and Collectivism: An fMRI Study. Social Neuroscience, 5, 257-271.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470910903374895 |
[15] | Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values (Vol. 5). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. |
[16] | Hong, Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural Minds: A Dynamic Constructivist Approach to Culture and Cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709 |
[17] | Jetten, J., Branscombe, N. R., & Spears, R. (2002). On Being Peripheral: Effects of Identity Insecurity on Personal and Collective Self-Esteem. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 105-123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.64 |
[18] | Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Mitchell, K. J., Touryan, S. R., Greene, E. J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2006). Dissociating Medial Frontal and Posterior Cingulate Activity during Self-Reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl004 |
[19] | Kagitcibasi, C. (2005). Autonomy and Relatedness in Cultural Context. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36, 403-422.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022105275959 |
[20] | Kashima, E. S., Hardie, E. A., Wakimoto, R., & Kashima, Y. (2011). Culture-and Gender-Specific Implications of Relational and Collective Contexts on Spontaneous Self-Descriptions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 740-758.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110362754 |
[21] | Kim, Y. H., Chiu, C. Y., Peng, S., Cai, H., & Tov, W. (2010). Explaining East-West Differences in the Likelihood of Making Favorable Self-Evaluations: The Role of Evaluation Ap-prehension and Directness of Expression. Journal of Cross-Cul- tural Psychology, 41, 62-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022109348921 |
[22] | Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and Collective Processes in the Construction of the Self: Self-Enhancement in the United States and Self-Criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1245-1267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1245 |
[23] | Kitayama, S., & Uchida, Y. (2003).Explicit Self-Criticism and Implicit Self-Regard: Evaluating Self and Friend in Two Cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 476-482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00026-X |
[24] | Lee, S. W. S., Oyserman, D., & Bond, M. H. (2010). Am I Doing Better Than you? That Depends on Whether You Ask Me in English or Chinese: Self-Enhancement Effects of Language as a Cultural Mindset Prime. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 785-791. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.04.005 |
[25] | Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the Self: Im-plications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224-253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 |
[26] | Matsumoto, D. (1999). Culture and Self: An Empirical Assess-ment of Markus and Kitayama’s Theory of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals. Asian Journal of Social Psy-chology, 2, 289-310.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-839X.00042 |
[27] | Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is There a Universal Positivity Bias in Attributions? A Meta-Analytic Review of Individual, Developmental, and Cultural Differences in the Self-Serving Attributional Bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 711-747. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711 |
[28] | Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissoci-able Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others. Neuron, 50, 655-663. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040 |
[29] | Ng, S. H., Han, S., Mao, L., & Lai, J. C. L. (2010). Dynamic Bicultural Brains: fMRI Study of Their Flexible Neural Representation of Self and Significant Others in Response to Culture Primes.Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 83-91.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2010.01303.x |
[30] | Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-Referential Processing in Our Brain—A Meta-Analysis of Imaging Studies on the Self. Neuroimage, 31, 440-457.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002 |
[31] | Oyserman, D., & Sorensen, N. (2009). Understanding Cul-tural Syndrome Effects on What and How We Think: A Situated Cognition Model. In R. S. Wyer, C.-Y. Chiu, & Y.-Y. Hong (Eds.), Understanding Culture: Theory, Research, and Application (pp. 25-52). New York, NY: Psychology Press. |
[32] | Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking Individualism and Collectivism: Evalu-ation of Theoretical Assumptions and Meta-Analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3 |
[33] | Panksepp, J., & Northoff, G. (2009). The Trans-Species Core SELF: The Emergence of Active Cultural and Neuro-Ecolog- ical Agents through Self-Related Processing within Subcor-tical-Cortical Midline Networks. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 193-215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.002 |
[34] | Pirttil-Backman, A. M., Kassea, B. R., & Ikonen, T. (2004). Cameroonian Forms of Collectivism and Individualism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 481-498. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022104266110 |
[35] | Realo, A., & Allik, J. (1999). A Cross-Cultural Study of Collec-tivism: A Comparison of American, Estonian, and Russian Students. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 133-142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224549909598367 |
[36] | Rosenberg, M. (Ed.) (1965). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. |
[37] | Santamaría, A., de la Mata, M. L., Hansen, T. G. B., & Ruiz, L. (2010). Cultural Self-Construals of Mexican, Spanish, and Danish College Students: Beyond Independent and Interde-pendent Self. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 471- 477. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022109359694 |
[38] | Sato, T., & Cameron, J. E. (1999). The Relationship between Collective Self-Esteem and Self-Construal in Japan and Canada. The Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 426-435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224549909598402 |
[39] | Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2005). Individualism: A Valid and Important Dimension of Cultural Differences between Nations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 17-31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0901_2 |
[40] | Schmitt, D. P., & Allik, J. (2005). Simultaneous Ad-ministration of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale in 53 Nations: Exploring the Universal and Culture-Specific Features of Global Self-Esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 623-642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.623 |
[41] | Sedikides, C., & Brewer, M. B. (2001). Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. |
[42] | Somech, A. (2000). The Independent and the Interdependent Selves: Different Meanings in Different Cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 161-172. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0147-1767(99)00030-9 |
[43] | Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-Construal Priming Modulates Neural Substrates of Self-Awareness. Psychological Science, 18, 861-866. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.x |
[44] | Sui, J., Zhu, Y., & Chiu, C. (2007). Bicultural Mind, Self-Construal, and Self-and Mother-Reference Effects: Consequences of Cultural Priming on Recognition Memory. Journal of Experimental social Psychology, 43, 818-824.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.08.005 |
[45] | Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some Tests of the Distinction between the Private Self and the Collective Self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649-655. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649 |
[46] | Tafarodi, R. W., Lang, J. M., & Smith, A. J. (1999). Self-Esteem and the Cultural Trade-Off. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 620-640. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022199030005004 |
[47] | Tafarodi, R. W., Shaughnessy, S. C., Yamaguchi, S., & Mu-rakoshi, A. (2011). The Reporting of Self-Esteem in Japan and Canada. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 155-164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022110386373 |
[48] | Tafarodi, R.W., & Walters, P. (1999). Individualism-Collectivism, Life Events, and Self-Esteem: A Test of Two Trade-Offs. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 797-814.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199908/09)29:5/6<797::AID-EJSP961>3.0.CO;2-S |
[49] | Tafarodi, R. W., & Swann, W. B. (1995). Self-Linking and Self-Competence as Dimensions of Global Self-Esteem: Initial Validation of a Measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 322-342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6502_8 |
[50] | Tam, H. C. (2009). A Comparison between Self-Competence and Self-Liking: Effects of Individualism-Collectivism Cultural Priming and Sense of Achievement. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong. |
[51] | Triandis, H. C. (1989). The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506-520.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506 |
[52] | Trompenaars, F. (1994). Riding the Waves of Culture: Under-standing Diversity in Global Business. Irwin Professional Pub. |
[53] | Yamaguchi, S., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Murakami, F., Chen, D., Shiomura, K. et al. (2007). Apparent Universality of Positive Implicit Self-Esteem. Psychological Science, 18, 498-500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01928.x |
[54] | Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. (2007). Neural Basis of Cultural Influence on Self-Representation.Neuroimage, 34, 1310-1316. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047 |