On the basis of postulates derived from cognitive-behavioral theory, research and therapy, the authors explored the extent to which older adults' cognitive beliefs of a just world and their perspectives on future time and similarity or self-continuity with the future self are predictors of long-term survival. After baseline assessment of health and cognitive beliefs and future perspectives of time and self-continuity as predictors of mortality, 440 participants (ages 65 to 87) were followed longitudinally for 6.5 years. Consistent with our hypotheses, findings demonstrated that a significantly higher percentage of survivors were individuals who showed higher scores on beliefs in a just world and on both the future time perspective and the future self-continuity perspective at the time of baseline assessments. Conversely, mortality risk was much higher for individuals who scored low on these predictor variables, and high on distrust. Implications for health and longevity are discussed. 1. Introduction To date, one of the most understudied etiologies of older adults’ survival and longevity has been the role of their cognitive beliefs and worldviews that possibly interact with their functional and mental capacities to endure, challenge, overcome, and survive in the face of the numerous struggles and obstacles in advanced old age. In earlier research on predictors of mortality, the focus has been exclusively on variations in physical health and sociodemographic variables to explain and predict differences in longevity and mortality rates across a wide age range. More recently, studies have explored the relationship between the 5-factor personality traits [1–4] and other stress-inducing traits of perfectionism and dysfunctional dependency traits to predict greater longevity or increased risk of mortality [5]. The present study presents a clear departure from earlier studies that have focused on sociodemographic and personality factors to explain and predict differences in all-cause mortality rates in later life. The goal of the present research is to move outside the personality and traits model to other second-order cognitive-behavioral factors to predict differences in all-cause mortality rates in later life. Cognitive-behavioral theorists argue on both theoretical and empirical grounds that individuals’ cognitive beliefs exert a great deal of influence on their health, resilience, and longevity and may logically be assumed to be robust predictors of impending mortality or conversely of longevity. However, the ability of cognitive belief systems to predict
References
[1]
B. W. Roberts, N. R. Kuncel, R. Shiner, A. Caspi, and L. R. Goldberg, “The power of personality: the comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes,” Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 313–345, 2007.
[2]
H. S. Friedman, J. S. Tucker, J. E. Schwartz et al., “Psychosocial and behavioral predictors of longevity: the aging and death of the “termites”,” American Psychologist, vol. 50, no. 2, pp. 69–78, 1995.
[3]
P. S. Fry and D. L. Debats, “Perfectionism and the five-factor personality traits as predictors of mortality in older adults,” Journal of Health Psychology, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 513–524, 2009.
[4]
R. D. Goodwin and H. S. Friedman, “Health status and the five-factor personality traits in a nationally representative sample,” Journal of Health Psychology, vol. 11, no. 5, pp. 643–654, 2006.
[5]
P. S. Fry and D. L. Debats, “Perfectionism and other related trait measures as predictors of mortality in diabetic older adults: a six-and-a half-year longitudinal study,” Journal of Health Psychology. In press. The online version of this article can be found at http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/03/26/1359105311398684.
[6]
L. R. Martin, H. S. Friedman, J. S. Tucker et al., “An archival prospective study of mental health and longevity,” Health Psychology, vol. 14, no. 5, pp. 381–387, 1995.
[7]
C. Dalbert, The Justice Motive as a Personal Resource: Dealing with Challenges and Critical Life Events, Plenum Press/ Kluwer Academic Publishers, New York, NY, USA, 2001.
[8]
N. T. Feather, “Human values, global self-esteem, and belief in a just world,” Journal of Personality, vol. 59, no. 1, pp. 83–107, 1991.
[9]
I. M. Lipkus and I. C. Siegler, “The belief in a just world and perceptions of discrimination,” The Journal of psychology, vol. 127, no. 4, pp. 465–474, 1993.
[10]
I. M. Lipkus, C. Dalbert, and I. C. Siegler, “The importance of distinguishing the belief in a just world for self versus for others: implications for psychological well-being,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 22, no. 7, pp. 666–677, 1996.
[11]
L. L. Carstensen, “The influence of a sense of time on human development,” Science, vol. 312, no. 5782, pp. 1913–1915, 2006.
[12]
S. Frederick, Discounting, time preference, and identity, dissertation, Department of Social and Decision Sciences. Carnegie-Mellon University, 1999.
[13]
H. Ersner-Hershfield, M. T. Garton, K. Ballard, G. R. Samanez-Larkin, and B. Knutson, “Don't stop thinking about tomorrow: individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving,” Judgment and Decision Making, vol. 4, no. 4, pp. 280–286, 2009.
[14]
C. L. Hafer and L. Bègue, “Experimental research on just-world theory: problems, developments, and future challenges,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 131, no. 1, pp. 128–167, 2005.
[15]
J. Tomaka and J. Blascovich, “Effects of justice beliefs on cognitive appraisals of subjective, physiological, and behavioral responses to potential stress,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 67, no. 4, pp. 732–740, 1994.
[16]
C. Ritter, D. E. Bensen, and C. Snyder, “Belief in a just world and depression,” Sociological Perspectives, vol. 33, pp. 235–252, 1990.
[17]
W. H. Jones, J. E. Freemon, and R. A. Goswick, “The persistence of loneliness: self and other determinants,” Journal of Personality, vol. 49, pp. 27–48, 1981.
[18]
D. Paulhus, “Sphere-specific measures of perceived control,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 44, no. 6, pp. 1253–1265, 1983.
[19]
J. B. Rotter, “A new scale for the measurement of interpersonal trust,” Journal of personality, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 651–665, 1967.
[20]
M. J. Lerner and D. T. Miller, “Just world research and the attribution process: looking back and ahead,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 85, no. 5, pp. 1030–1051, 1978.
[21]
C. L. Hafer and J. M. Olson, “Beliefs in a just world and reactions to personal deprivation,” Journal of Personality, vol. 57, pp. 799–823, 1989.
[22]
S. E. Taylor and J. D. Brown, “Illusion and well-being: a social psychological perspective on mental health,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 103, no. 2, pp. 193–210, 1988.
[23]
L. Bègue and M. Bastounis, “Two spheres of belief in justice: extensive support for the bi-dimensional model of belief in a just world,” Journal of Personality, vol. 71, no. 3, pp. 435–463, 2003.
[24]
R. J. Bulman and C. B. Wortman, “Attributions of blame and coping in the “real world”: severe accident victims react to their lot,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 351–363, 1977.
[25]
F. R. Lang and L. L. Carstensen, “Time counts: future time perspective, goals, and social relationships,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 125–139, 2002.
[26]
L. L. Carstensen, Future Time Perspective Scale, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif, USA, 1996.
[27]
A. Aron, E. N. Aron, and D. Smollan, “Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 63, no. 4, pp. 596–612, 1992.
[28]
M. Rosenberg, C. Schooler, C. Schoenbach, and F. Rosenberg, “Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: different concepts, different outcomes,” American Sociological Review, vol. 60, pp. 141–156, 1995.
[29]
G. D. Zimet, N. W. Dahlem, S. G. Zimet, and G. K. Farley, “The multidimensional scale of perceived social support,” Journal of Personality Assessment, vol. 52, pp. 30–41, 1988.
[30]
D. R. Cox, “Regression models and life tables,” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B, vol. 74, pp. 187–220, 1972.
[31]
J. L. Kelsey, A. S. Whittmore, A. Evans, and W. D. Thompson, Methods in Observational Epidemiology, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[32]
P. S. Fry and D. L. Debats, “Psychosocial resources as predictors of resilience and healthy longevity of older widows,” in New Frontiers in Resilient Aging: Life-Strengths and Well Being in Later Life, P. S. Fry and C. L. M. Keyes, Eds., pp. 185–212, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 2010.
[33]
A. E. Korten, A. F. Jorm, Z. Jiao et al., “Health, cognitive, and psychosocial factors as predictors of mortality in an elderly community sample,” Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, vol. 53, no. 2, pp. 83–88, 1999.
[34]
J. D. Kalbfleisch and R. L. Prentice, Statistical analysis of failure time data, Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 1980.
[35]
A. Weiss and P. T. Costa, “Domain and facet personality predictors of all-cause mortality among medicare patients aged 65 to 100,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 67, no. 5, pp. 724–733, 2005.
[36]
D. Parfit, “Personal identity,” Philosophical Review, vol. 80, pp. 3–27, 1971.
[37]
G. D. Mellinger, “Interpersonal trust as a factor in communication,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 52, no. 3, pp. 304–309, 1956.
[38]
P. S. Fry and D. L. Debats, “Sources of life strengths as predictors of late-life mortality and survivorship,” International Journal of Aging and Human Development, vol. 62, no. 4, pp. 303–334, 2006.