Background. Community mobility is crucial for maintaining independent functioning and quality of life for older adults. Purpose. The present paper describes the relationship of cognition, particularly speed of processing as measured by the Useful Field of View Test, to mobility as indicated by driving behaviors, life space, and falls among healthy older adults. Research examining the impact of cognitive speed of processing training (SOPT) on older adults' community mobility (i.e., driving behaviors) is also summarized. Key Issues. Even slight cognitive declines can place older adults at risk for mobility limitations. However, cognitive interventions like SOPT can mitigate declines in driving mobility. Implications. The potential of SOPT to sustain community mobility among older adults is discussed. 1. Introduction Mobility may be defined as the ability to move through one’s environment in order to complete a task or achieve a goal [1, 2]. Continued mobility is crucial for maintaining independent functioning and quality of life [3, 4]. Yet, mobility limitations increase with age [3]. Decreased mobility among older adults, defined in this paper as individuals 55 years of age and older, may result from numerous factors such as failing health [3] and vision [5]. Age-related declines in aspects of cognition, including memory, reasoning, executive functioning, and speed of processing [6], may also predict mobility among community-dwelling older adults without dementia [7]. The Useful Field of View Test (UFOV, a registered trademark of Visual Awareness, Inc.) is a cognitive measure of visual processing speed for attentional tasks [8, 9] that has consistently emerged as a predictor of mobility. This paper describes the relationship of cognitive speed of processing, as measured by UFOV, to community mobility as indicated by driving, life space, and the occurrence of falls. The relationship of UFOV to mobility is of particular interest in that UFOV difficulties can be rehabilitated with training [10–12]. Research has indicated that speed of processing training (SOPT), a cognitive intervention, not only improves speed of processing but also transfers to prolonged safe driving mobility among older adults [13, 14]. Research examining the impact of this intervention on older adults’ driving mobility is summarized. The potential of such cognitive interventions to sustain mobility and quality of life among older adults is discussed. 2. Driving, Life Space, and Falls Driving is an important aspect of community mobility, particularly for older adults in the United States
References
[1]
B. T. Stalvey, C. Owsley, M. E. Sloane, and K. Ball, “The life space questionnaire: a measure of the extent of mobility of older adults,” Journal of Applied Gerontology, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 460–478, 1999.
[2]
G. P. Barberger and C. Fabrigoule, “Disability and cognitive impairment in the elderly,” Disability and Rehabilitation, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 175–193, 1997.
[3]
J. M. Guralnik, A. Z. LaCroix, R. D. Abbott, et al., “Maintaining mobility in late life. I. Demographic characteristics and chronic conditions,” American Journal of Epidemiology, vol. 137, no. 8, pp. 845–857, 1993.
[4]
R. A. Marottoli, L. M. Cooney, D. R. Wagner, J. Doucette, and M. E. Tinetti, “Predictors of automobile crashes and moving violations among elderly drivers,” Annals of Internal Medicine, vol. 121, no. 11, pp. 842–846, 1994.
[5]
C. Owsley, B. Stalvey, J. Wells, and M. E. Sloane, “Older drivers and cataract: driving habits and crash risk,” The Journals of Gerontology A, vol. 54, no. 4, pp. M203–M211, 1999.
[6]
H. C. Hendrie, “The NIH cognitive and emotional health project: report of the critical evaluation study committee,” lzheimer's & Dementia, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 12–32, 2006.
[7]
K. M. Wood, J. D. Edwards, O. J. Clay, V. G. Wadley, D. L. Roenker, and K. K. Ball, “Sensory and cognitive factors influencing functional ability in older adults,” Gerontology, vol. 51, no. 2, pp. 131–141, 2005.
[8]
J. D. Edwards, L. A. Ross, V. G. Wadley, et al., “The useful field of view test: normative data for older adults,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 275–286, 2006.
[9]
J. D. Edwards, D. E. Vance, V. G. Wadley, G. M. Cissell, D. L. Roenker, and K. K. Ball, “Reliability and validity of useful field of view test scores as administered by personal computer,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 529–543, 2005.
[10]
K. Ball, D. B. Berch, K. F. Helmers, et al., “Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 288, no. 18, pp. 2271–2281, 2002.
[11]
J. D. Edwards, V. G. Wadley, R. S. Myers, D. L. Roenker, G. M. Cissell, and K. K. Ball, “Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions,” Gerontology, vol. 48, no. 5, pp. 329–340, 2002.
[12]
J. D. Edwards, V. G. Wadley, D. E. Vance, K. Wood, D. L. Roenker, and K. K. Ball, “The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance,” Aging & Mental Health, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 262–271, 2005.
[13]
J. D. Edwards, P. B. Delahunt, and H. W. Mahncke, “Cognitive speed of processing training delays driving cessation,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 64, no. 12, pp. 1262–1267, 2009.
[14]
J. D. Edwards, C. Myers, L. A. Ross, et al., “The longitudinal impact of cognitive speed of processing training on driving mobility,” The Gerontologist, vol. 49, no. 4, pp. 485–494, 2009.
[15]
D. O'Neill, “Safe mobility for older people,” Reviews in Clinical Gerontology, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 181–191, 2000.
[16]
R. A. Marottoli, C. F. M. de Leon, T. A. Glass, C. S. Williams, L. M. Cooney Jr., and L. F. Berkman, “Consequences of driving cessation: decreased out-of-home activity levels,” The Journals of Gerontology, vol. 55, no. 6, pp. S334–S340, 2000.
[17]
G. McGwin Jr., V. Chapman, and C. Owsley, “Visual risk factors for driving difficulty among older drivers,” Accident Analysis and Prevention, vol. 32, no. 6, pp. 735–744, 2000.
[18]
K. K. Ball, D. L. Roenker, V. G. Wadley, et al., “Can high-risk older drivers be identified through performance-based measures in a department of motor vehicles setting?” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 77–84, 2006.
[19]
J. D. Edwards, E. Bart, M. L. O'Connor, and G. Cissell, “Ten years down the road: predictors of driving cessation,” Gerontologist, vol. 50, no. 3, pp. 393–399, 2010.
[20]
J. D. Edwards, L. A. Ross, M. L. Ackerman, et al., “Longitudinal predictors of driving cessation among older adults from the ACTIVE clinical trial,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 63, no. 1, pp. P6–P12, 2008.
[21]
O. J. Clay, V. G. Wadley, J. D. Edwards, D. L. Roth, D. L. Roenker, and K. K. Ball, “Cumulative meta-analysis of the relationship between useful field of view and driving performance in older adults: current and future implications,” Optometry and Vision Science, vol. 82, no. 8, pp. 724–731, 2005.
[22]
P. S. Baker, E. V. Bodner, and R. M. Allman, “Measuring life-space mobility in community-dwelling older adults,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 51, no. 11, pp. 1610–1614, 2003.
[23]
L. Clemson, D. Manor, and M. H. Fitzgerald, “Behavioral factors contributing to older adults falling in public places,” OTJR Occupation, Participation and Health, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 107–117, 2003.
[24]
L. Mackenzie, J. Byles, and N. Higginbotham, “A prospective community-based study of falls among older people in Australia: frequency, circumstances, and consequences,” OTJR Occupation, Participation and Health, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 143–152, 2002.
[25]
A. J. Campbell, M. C. Robertson, M. M. Gardner, R. N. Norton, and D. M. Buchner, “Psychotropic medication withdrawal and a home-based exercise program to prevent falls: a randomized, controlled trial,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 47, no. 7, pp. 850–853, 1999.
[26]
S. R. Lord, J. Dayhew, and A. Howland, “Multifocal glasses impair edge-contrast sensitivity and depth perception and increase the risk of falls in older people,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 50, no. 11, pp. 1760–1766, 2002.
[27]
D. E. Vance, K. K. Ball, D. L. Roenker, V. G. Wadley, J. D. Edwards, and G. M. Cissell, “Predictors of falling in older Maryland drivers: a structural-equation model,” Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 254–269, 2006.
[28]
D. X. Rasmusson, G. W. Rebok, F. W. Bylsma, and J. Brandt, “Effects of three types of memory training in normal elderly,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 56–66, 1999.
[29]
A. F. Kramer, J. F. Larish, and D. L. Strayer, “Training for attentional control in dual task settings: a comparison of young and older adults,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 50–76, 1995.
[30]
K. W. Schaie, S. L. Willis, C. Hertzog, and J. E. Schulenberg, “Effects of cognitive training on primary mental ability structure,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 233–242, 1987.
[31]
F. W. Unverzagt, D. M. Smith, G. W. Rebok, et al., “The Indiana Alzheimer Disease Center's symposium on mild cognitive impairment. Cognitive training in older adults: lessons from the ACTIVE study,” Current Alzheimer Research, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 375–383, 2009.
[32]
D. E. Vance, J. Dawson, V. Wadley, et al., “The accelerate study: the longitudinal effect of speed of processing training on cognitive performance of older adults,” Rehabilitation Psychology, vol. 52, no. 1, pp. 89–96, 2007.
[33]
V. G. Wadley, R. L. Benz, K. K. Ball, D. L. Roenker, J. D. Edwards, and D. E. Vance, “Development and evaluation of home-based speed-of-processing training for older adults,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 87, no. 6, pp. 757–763, 2006.
[34]
D. E. Vance, K. Heaton, P. L. Fazeli, and M. L. Ackerman, “Aging, speed of processing training, and everyday functioning: implications for practice and research,” Activities, Adaptation and Aging, vol. 34, no. 4, pp. 276–291, 2010.
[35]
K. Ball, J. D. Edwards, and L. A. Ross, “The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday functions,” The Journals of Gerontology, vol. 62B, pp. 19–31, 2007.
[36]
K. K. Ball, C. Owsley, B. Stalvey, D. L. Roenker, M. E. Sloane, and M. Graves, “Driving avoidance and functional impairment in older drivers,” Accident Analysis and Prevention, vol. 30, no. 3, pp. 313–322, 1998.
[37]
D. L. Roenker, G. M. Cissell, K. K. Ball, V. G. Wadley, and J. D. Edwards, “Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance,” Human Factors, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 218–233, 2003.
[38]
J. B. Jobe, D. M. Smith, K. Ball, et al., “Active: a cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults,” Controlled Clinical Trials, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 453–479, 2001.
[39]
S. L. Willis, S. L. Tennstedt, M. Marsiske, et al., “Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 296, no. 23, pp. 2805–2814, 2006.
[40]
L. A. Ross, Does speed of processing training impact driving mobility in older adults? Ph.D. thesis, University of Alabama, Birmingham, Ala, USA, 2008.
[41]
K. K. Ball, J. D. Edwards, L. A. Ross, and G. McGwin, “Cognitive training decreases motor vehicle collision involvement of older drivers,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 58, no. 11, pp. 2107–2113, 2010.
[42]
F. D. Wolinsky, F. W. Unverzagt, D. M. Smith, R. Jones, A. Stoddard, and S. L. Tennstedt, “The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: protection that lasts for 5 years,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 61, no. 12, pp. 1324–1329, 2006.
[43]
F. D. Wolinsky, H. Mahncke, M. W. Vander Weg, et al., “Speed of processing training protects self-rated health in older adults: enduring effects observed in the multi-site ACTIVE randomized controlled trial,” International Psychogeriatrics, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 470–478, 2010.
[44]
F. D. Wolinsky, H. W. Mahncke, M. Kosinski, et al., “The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and predicted medical expenditures,” in Proceedings of the Transportation Research Board Annual Meeting, National Acadamies of Science, Washington, DC, USA, 2009.
[45]
F. D. Wolinsky, M. W. Vander Weg, R. Martin, et al., “The effect of speed-of-processing training on depressive symptoms in ACTIVE,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 64A, no. 4, pp. 468–472, 2009.