This study examined how MTBI concomitants such as pain variables, depression, and anxiety were related to attentional functioning at different stages of recovery. Participants having sustained a MTBI who were in the earlier phase of recovery showed, compared to controls, slower reaction times and larger intra-individual variability on a Computerized Pictorial Stroop Task (CPST). They also reported more post-concussion symptoms, pain intensity and disability, whereas MTBI participants who were in the later phase of recovery presented a higher rate of post-concussive symptoms and somewhat higher pain intensity/disability. MTBI participants’ scores on the cognitive items of the post-concussion symptoms scale were positively correlated with reaction times on the CPST, while pain intensity/disability levels were negatively correlated with standard attention measures. Results indicate that obtaining response times and intra-individual variability measures using tests such as the CPST represents an effective means for measuring recovery of attentional function, and that pain intensity/disability should be systematically assessed after a MTBI. 1. Introduction Individuals who have sustained a mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI) may manifest postconcussional symptoms of a physical, cognitive, or emotional nature [1, 2]. Possible cognitive symptoms include difficulties with concentration, attention, memory, executive functioning [3], word finding, and information processing [4]. Therefore, the cognitive impact of MTBI can be extensive and wide ranging [5]. It is well known that of the reported symptoms, attention is especially problematic for many individuals having had a MTBI. In fact, meta-analytical studies indicate that attention deficits are the most persistent neuropsychological complaint following closed-head injury [6, 7]. Understanding these attentional difficulties is important in planning management and rehabilitation of persons suffering from the consequences of MTBI. Multiple studies have demonstrated that MTBI produces attentional deficits [8, 9]. In fact, there appears to be a large variety of attentional deficits found within this group of patients in terms of both reported symptoms and neuropsychological performance [5]. Divided attention deficits and sustained attention deficits have been identified in the MTBI population [10–12]. As well, results of some experimental studies suggest that MTBI may produce difficulty in effectively filtering relevant sensory information from irrelevant information, or of selective attention [9, 13].
References
[1]
I. Emanuelson, E. Andersson Holmkvist, R. Bj?rklund, and D. St?lhammar, “Quality of life and post-concussion symptoms in adults after mild traumatic brain injury: a population-based study in western Sweden,” Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, vol. 108, no. 5, pp. 332–338, 2003.
[2]
A. Sterr, K. A. Herron, C. Hayward, and D. Montaldi, “Are mild head injuries as mild as we think? Neurobehavioral concomitants of chronic post-concussion syndrome,” BMC Neurology, vol. 6, article 7, 2006.
[3]
D. B. Arciniegas, C. A. Anderson, J. Topkoff, and T. W. McAllister, “Mild traumatic brain injury: a neuropsychiatric approach to diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment,” Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 311–327, 2005.
[4]
D. Kushner, “Mild traumatic brain injury: toward understanding manifestations and treatment,” Archives of Internal Medicine, vol. 158, no. 15, pp. 1617–1624, 1998.
[5]
R. C. K. Chan, “Attentional deficits in patients with closed head injury: a further study to the discriminative validity of the test of everyday attention,” Brain Injury, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 227–236, 2000.
[6]
L. M. Binder, “A review of mild head trauma. Part II: clinical implications,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 432–457, 1997.
[7]
L. M. Binder, M. L. Rohling, and G. J. Larrabee, “A review of mild head trauma. Part I: meta-analytic review of neuropsychological studies,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 421–431, 1997.
[8]
N. Landre, C. J. Poppe, N. Davis, B. Schmaus, and S. E. Hobbs, “Cognitive functioning and postconcussive symptoms in trauma patients with and without mild TBI,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 255–273, 2006.
[9]
N. Bohnen, J. Jolles, and A. Twijnstra, “Modification of the Stroop Color Word Test improves differentiation between patients with mild head injury and matched controls,” Clinical Neuropsychologist, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 178–184, 1992.
[10]
N. Paré, L. A. Rabin, J. Fogel, and M. Pépin, “Mild traumatic brain injury and its sequelae: characterisation of divided attention deficits,” Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 110–137, 2009.
[11]
K. D. Cicerone, “Attention deficits and dual task demands after mild traumatic brain injury,” Brain Injury, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 79–89, 1996.
[12]
R. C. K. Chan, “Sustained attention in patients with mild traumatic brain injury,” Clinical Rehabilitation, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 188–193, 2005.
[13]
C. Ziino and J. Ponsford, “Selective attention deficits and subjective fatigue following traumatic brain injury,” Neuropsychology, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 383–390, 2006.
[14]
M. Gaetz, “The neurophysiology of brain injury,” Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 115, no. 1, pp. 4–18, 2004.
[15]
M. P. Alexander, “Mild traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, natural history, and clinical management,” Neurology, vol. 45, no. 7, pp. 1253–1260, 1995.
[16]
R. H. Singleton and J. T. Povlishock, “Identification and characterization of heterogeneous neuronal injury and death in regions of diffuse brain injury: evidence for multiple independent injury phenotypes,” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 24, no. 14, pp. 3543–3553, 2004.
[17]
M. L. Prins, J. T. Povlishock, and L. L. Phillips, “The effects of combined fluid percussion traumatic brain injury and unilateral entorhinal deafferentation on the juvenile rat brain,” Developmental Brain Research, vol. 140, no. 1, pp. 93–104, 2003.
[18]
G. Barkhoudarian, D. A. Hovda, and C. C. Giza, “The molecular phathophysiology of concussive brain injury,” Clinics in Sports Medicine, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 33–48, 2011.
[19]
M. E. Selzer, “Introduction: epidemiology and pathophysiology of traumatic brain injury,” Journal of Neurologic Rehabilitation, vol. 9, no. 2, pp. 55–60, 1995.
[20]
T. Shallice, From Neuropsychology to Mental Status, Cambridge University Press, 1990.
[21]
R. L. Wood, “Post concussional syndrome: all in the minds eye!,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 78, no. 6, artticle 552, 2007.
[22]
J. Sheedy, E. Harvey, S. Faux, G. Geffen, and E. A. Shores, “Emergency department assessment of mild traumatic brain injury and the prediction of postconcussive symptoms: a 3-month prospective study,” Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, vol. 24, no. 5, pp. 333–343, 2009.
[23]
J. R. Fann, W. J. Katon, J. M. Uomoto, and P. C. Esselman, “Psychiatric disorders and functional disability in outpatients with traumatic brain injuries,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 152, no. 10, pp. 1493–1499, 1995.
[24]
E. M. S. Sherman, E. Strauss, D. J. Slick, and F. Spellacy, “Effect of depression on neuropsychological functioning in head injury: measurable but minimal,” Brain Injury, vol. 14, no. 7, pp. 621–632, 2000.
[25]
C. S. Gass, “MMPI-2 variables in attention and memory test performance,” Psychological Assessment, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 135–138, 1996.
[26]
D. E. Nampiaparampil, “Prevalence of chronic pain after traumatic brain injury: a systematic review,” JAMA, vol. 300, no. 6, pp. 711–719, 2008.
[27]
J. M. Uomoto and P. C. Esselman, “Traumatic brain injury and chronic pain: differential types and rates by head injury severity,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 74, no. 1, pp. 61–64, 1993.
[28]
F. Guérin, S. Kennepohl, G. Léveillé, A. Dominique, and M. McKerral, “Vocational outcome indicators in atypically recovering mild TBI: a post-intervention study,” NeuroRehabilitation, vol. 21, no. 4, pp. 295–303, 2006.
[29]
R. P. Hart, M. F. Martelli, and N. D. Zasler, “Chronic pain and neuropsychological functioning,” Neuropsychology Review, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 131–149, 2000.
[30]
A. Khatibi, M. Dehghani, L. Sharpe, G. J. G. Asmundson, and H. Pouretemad, “Selective attention towards painful faces among chronic pain patients: evidence from a modified version of the dot-probe,” Pain, vol. 142, no. 1-2, pp. 42–47, 2009.
[31]
G. Crombez, S. Van Damme, and C. Eccleston, “Hypervigilance to pain: an experimental and clinical analysis,” Pain, vol. 116, no. 1-2, pp. 4–7, 2005.
[32]
C. Eccleston and G. Crombez, “Pain demands attention: a cognitive-affective model of the interruptive function of pain,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 125, no. 3, pp. 356–366, 1999.
[33]
R. Vernon-Wilkinson and H. Tuokko, “The influence of pain symptoms on neuropsychological test scores,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 9, article 2, 1993.
[34]
V. Rao, M. Bertrand, P. Rosenberg et al., “Predictors of new-onset depression after mild traumatic brain injury,” Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 100–104, 2010.
[35]
R. Whelan-Goodinson, J. Ponsford, and M. Sch?nberger, “Association between psychiatric state and outcome following traumatic brain injury,” Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, vol. 40, no. 10, pp. 850–857, 2008.
[36]
T. Kay, D. E. Harrington, R. Adams et al., “Definition of mild traumatic brain injury,” Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 86–87, 1993.
[37]
A. T. Beck, R. A. Steer, and G. K. Brown, Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II, Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 1996.
[38]
A. T. Beck and R. A. Steer, Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) Manual, Pearson Assessment, Oxford, UK, 1990.
[39]
M. R. Lovell and M. W. Collins, “Neuropsychological assessment of the college football player,” Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 9–26, 1998.
[40]
M. R. Lovell, G. L. Iverson, M. W. Collins et al., “Measurement of symptoms following sports-related concussion: reliability and normative data for the post-concussion scale,” Applied Neuropsychology, vol. 13, no. 3, pp. 166–174, 2006.
[41]
R. Melzack, “The McGill pain questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods,” PAIN, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 277–299, 1975.
[42]
C. A. Pollard, “Preliminary validity study of the pain disability index,” Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol. 59, no. 3, artticle 974, 1984.
[43]
I. H. Robertson, T. Ward, V. Ridgeway, et al., The Test of Everyday Attention, Thames Valley Test Company, Bury St. Edmunds, UK, 1994.
[44]
R. M. Ruff and C. C. Allen, Ruff 2 & 7 Selective Attention Test Professional Manual, Psychological Assessment Resources, Lutz, Fla:, 1996.
[45]
J. Roelofs, M. L. Peters, M. P. A. Zeegers, and J. W. S. Vlaeyen, “The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain-related stimuli among chronic pain patients: a meta-analysis,” European Journal of Pain, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 273–281, 2002.
[46]
I. T. Kolassa, F. Musial, A. Mohr, R. H. Trippe, and W. H. R. Miltner, “Electrophysiological correlates of threat processing in spider phobics,” Psychophysiology, vol. 42, no. 5, pp. 520–530, 2005.
[47]
M. Kindt and J. F. Brosschot, “Cognitive bias in spider-phobic children: comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop,” Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 207–220, 1999.
[48]
S. Roy, C. Roy, I. Fortin, C. Ethier-Majcher, P. G. F. Belin, and F. Gosselin, “A dynamic facial expression data-base,” Journal of Vision, vol. 7, article 944, 2007.
[49]
P. L. Jackson, A. N. Meltzoff, and J. Decety, “How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy,” NeuroImage, vol. 24, no. 3, pp. 771–779, 2005.
[50]
D. T. Stuss, L. L. Stethem, H. Hugenholtz, T. Picton, J. Pivik, and M. T. Richard, “Reaction time after head injury: fatigue, divided and focused attention, and consistency of performance,” Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 52, no. 6, pp. 742–748, 1989.
[51]
J. Bleiberg, E. L. Halpern, D. Reeves, and J. C. Daniel, “Future directions for the neuropsychological assessment of sports concussion,” Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 36–44, 1998.
[52]
K. L. Felmingham, I. J. Baguley, and A. M. Green, “Effects of diffuse axonal injury on speed of information processing following severe traumatic brain injury,” Neuropsychology, vol. 18, no. 3, pp. 564–571, 2004.
[53]
D. T. Stuss, K. J. Murphy, M. A. Binns, and M. P. Alexander, “Staying on the job: the frontal lobes control individual performance variability,” Brain, vol. 126, no. 11, pp. 2363–2380, 2003.
[54]
D. T. Stuss, J. Pogue, L. Buckle, and J. Bondar, “Characterization of stability of performance in patients with traumatic brain injury: variability and consistency on reaction time tests,” Neuropsychology, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 316–324, 1994.
[55]
J. Whyte, M. Polansky, M. Fleming, H. B. Coslett, and C. Cavallucci, “Sustained arousal and attention after traumatic brain injury,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 33, no. 7, pp. 797–813, 1995.
[56]
S. J. Segalowitz, J. Dywan, and A. Unsal, “Attentional factors in response time variability after traumatic brain injury: an ERP study,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 95–107, 1997.
[57]
T. P. Zahn and A. F. Mirsky, “Reaction time indicators of attention deficits in closed head injury,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 352–367, 1999.
[58]
J. de Houwer and H. Tibboel, “Stop what you are not doing! emotional pictures interfere with the task not to respond,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 699–703, 2010.
[59]
U. Schimmack, “Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: threat, negativity, or arousal?” Emotion, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 55–66, 2005.
[60]
H. S. Levin, D. H. Williams, H. M. Eisenberg, J. W. M. High, and J. F. C. Guinto, “Serial MRI and neurobehavioural findings after mild to moderate closed head injury,” Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 55, no. 4, pp. 255–262, 1992.
[61]
H. S. Levin, E. Amparo, and H. M. Eisenberg, “Magnetic resonance imaging and computerized tomography in relation to the neurobehavioral sequelae of mild and moderate head injuries,” Journal of Neurosurgery, vol. 66, no. 5, pp. 706–713, 1987.
[62]
T. Kay, B. Newman, M. Cavallo, O. Ezrachi, and M. Resnick, “Toward a neuropsychological model of functional disability after mild traumatic brain injury,” Neuropsychology, vol. 6, no. 4, pp. 371–384, 1992.
[63]
A. Collie, D. Darby, and P. Maruff, “Computerised cognitive assessment of athletes with sports related head injury,” British Journal of Sports Medicine, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 297–302, 2001.
[64]
C. M. J. Braun, S. Daigneault, and D. Champagne, “information processing deficits as indexed by reaction time parameters in severe closed head injury,” International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 167–176, 1989.
[65]
C. R. Hetherington, D. T. Stuss, and M. A. J. Finlayson, “Reaction time and variability 5 and 10 years after traumatic brain injury,” Brain Injury, vol. 10, no. 7, pp. 473–486, 1996.
[66]
O. Godefroy, C. Lhullier, and M. Rousseaux, “Reliability of reaction time measurements in brain-damaged patients,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 126, no. 2, pp. 168–171, 1994.
[67]
T. N. Tombaugh, L. Rees, P. Stormer, A. G. Harrison, and A. Smith, “The effects of mild and severe traumatic brain injury on speed of information processing as measured by the computerized tests of information processing (CTIP),” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 22, no. 1, pp. 25–36, 2007.
[68]
N. S. King, “Emotional, neuropsychological, and organic factors: their use in the prediction of persisting postconcussion symptoms after moderate and mild head injuries,” Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 75–81, 1996.
[69]
S. Dikmen, J. Machamer, J. R. Fann, and N. R. Temkin, “Rates of symptom reporting following traumatic brain injury,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 401–411, 2010.