Subjects with life-threatening asthma (LTA) have reported decreased sensitivity to inspiratory resistive (R) loads. It is unknown if decreased sensitivity is specific for inspiratory R loads, other types of respiratory loads, or a general deficit affecting sensory modalities. This study hypothesized that impairment is specific to respiratory stimuli. This study tested perceptual sensitivity of LTA, asthmatic (A), and nonasthmatic (NA) subjects to 4 sensory modalities: respiratory, somatosensory, auditory, visual. Perceptual sensitivity was measured with magnitude estimation (ME): respiratory loads ME, determined using inspiratory R and pressure threshold (PT) loads; somatosensory ME, determined using weight ranges of 2–20?kg; auditory ME, determined using graded magnitudes of 1?kHz tones delivered for 3 seconds bilaterally; visual ME, determined using gray-to-white disk intensity gradations on black background. ME for inspiratory R loads lessened for LTA over A and NA subjects. There was no significant difference between the 3 groups in ME for PT inspiratory loads, weight, sound, and visual trials. These results demonstrate that LTA subjects are poor perceivers of inspiratory R loads. This deficit in respiratory perception is specific to inspiratory R loads and is not due to perceptual deficits in other types of inspiratory loads, somatosensory, auditory, or visual sensory modalities. 1. Introduction Asthma is a respiratory disease frequently diagnosed in childhood. To control and/or prevent an asthma attack, it is important for the patient to heed initial symptoms and to be compliant with their prescribed medication(s). Failure to recognize and self-manage of an asthma exacerbation is one cause of life-threatening asthma (LTA) [1–3]. Difficulty in perceiving asthma symptoms can be one of many factors causing the patient to fail to recognize the onset of an asthma attack [2, 4, 5]. A subpopulation of asthmatic patients with a history of LTA has been reported with reduced perception of both intrinsic and extrinsic respiratory loads [2, 4]. These LTA asthmatic patients have an increased threshold for detection of inspiratory resistive loads, a decreased ability to scale the magnitude of inspiratory loads and a decreased perception of intrinsic bronchoconstriction [2, 4]. It has also been reported that the somatosensory cortex is not activated by inspiratory loads in these LTA subjects suggesting a sensory neural deficit in respiratory information processing in this subpopulation of asthmatic patients [1]. While it is evident that these LTA subjects have
References
[1]
P. W. Davenport, M. Cruz, A. A. Stecenko, and Y. Kifle, “Respiratory-related evoked potentials in children with life-threatening asthma,” American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, vol. 161, no. 6, pp. 1830–1835, 2000.
[2]
Y. Kifle, V. Seng, and P. W. Davenport, “Magnitude estimation of inspiratory resistive loads in children with life-threatening asthma,” American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, vol. 156, no. 5, pp. 1530–1535, 1997.
[3]
Y. Kikuchi, W. Hida, T. Chonan, C. Shindoh, H. Sasaki, and T. Takishima, “Decrease in functional residual capacity during inspiratory loading and the sensation of dyspnea,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 71, no. 5, pp. 1787–1794, 1991.
[4]
S. M. Julius, K. L. Davenport, and P. W. Davenport, “Perception of intrinsic and extrinsic respiratory loads in children with life-threatening asthma,” Pediatric Pulmonology, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 425–433, 2002.
[5]
J. M. Sherman and C. L. Capen, “The red alert program for life-threatening asthma,” Pediatrics, vol. 100, no. 2, pp. 187–191, 1997.
[6]
J. H. C. M. Bakers and S. M. Tenney, “The perception of some sensations associated with breathing,” Respiration Physiology, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 85–92, 1970.
[7]
I. D. Bijl-Hofland, S. G. M. Cloosterman, C. P. van Schayck, F. J. J. V. D. Elshout, R. P. Akkermans, and H. T. M. Folgering, “Perception of respiratory sensation assessed by means of histamine challenge and threshold loading tests,” Chest, vol. 117, no. 4, pp. 954–959, 2000.
[8]
J. G. W. Burdon, E. F. Juniper, and K. J. Killian, “The perception of breathlessness in asthma,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 126, no. 5, pp. 825–828, 1982.
[9]
N. K. Burki, “Effects of bronchodilation on magnitude estimation of added resistive loads in asthmatic subjects,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 129, no. 2, pp. 225–229, 1984.
[10]
N. K. Burki, P. W. Davenport, F. Safdar, and F. W. Zechman, “The effects of airway anesthesia on magnitude estimation of added inspiratory resistive and elastic loads,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 127, no. 1, pp. 2–4, 1983.
[11]
N. K. Burki, K. Mitchell, B. A. Chaudhary, and F. W. Zechman, “The ability of asthmatics to detect added resistive loads,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 117, no. 1, pp. 71–75, 1978.
[12]
K. R. Chapman and A. S. Rebuck, “Inspiratory and expiratory resistive loading as a model of dyspnea in asthma,” Respiration, vol. 44, no. 6, pp. 425–432, 1983.
[13]
J. A. Gliner, L. J. Folinsbee, and S. M. Horvath, “Accuracy and precision of matching inspired lung volume,” Perception and Psychophysics, vol. 29, no. 5, pp. 511–515, 1981.
[14]
S. B. Gottfried, M. D. Altose, S. G. Kelsen, and N. S. Cherniack, “Perception of changes in airflow resistance in obstructive pulmonary disorders,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 124, no. 5, pp. 566–570, 1981.
[15]
I. Homma, “Inspiratory inhibitory reflex caused by the chest wall vibration in man,” Respiration Physiology, vol. 39, no. 3, pp. 345–353, 1980.
[16]
C. H. Huang, A. D. Martin, and P. W. Davenport, “Effects of inspiratory strength training on the detection of inspiratory loads,” Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 17–26, 2009.
[17]
S. G. Kelsen, T. F. Prestel, and N. S. Cherniack, “Comparison of the respiratory responses to external resistive loading and bronchoconstriction,” Journal of Clinical Investigation, vol. 67, no. 6, pp. 1761–1768, 1981.
[18]
K. J. Killian, C. K. Mahutte, and E. J. M. Campbell, “Magnitude scaling of externally added loads to breathing,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 123, no. 1, pp. 12–15, 1981.
[19]
R. W. Lansing and R. B. Banzett, “Psychophysical methods in the study of respiratory sensation,” in Respiratory Sensation, L. Adams and A. Guz, Eds., vol. 90 of Lung Biology in Health and Disease, pp. 69–100, Marcel Dekker, New York, NY, USA, 1996.
[20]
M. L. Moy, J. W. Weiss, D. Sparrow, E. Israel, and R. M. Schwartzstein, “Quality of dyspnea in bronchoconstriction differs from external resistive loads,” American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine, vol. 162, no. 2, pp. 451–455, 2000.
[21]
W. R. Revelette and R. L. Wiley, “Plasticity of the mechanism subserving inspiratory load perception,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 62, no. 5, pp. 1901–1906, 1987.
[22]
O. Taguchi, Y. Kikuchi, W. Hida et al., “Effects of bronchoconstriction and external resistive loading on the sensation of dyspnea,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 71, no. 6, pp. 2183–2190, 1991.
[23]
F. W. Zechman and R. Wiley, “Afferent inputs to breathing: respiratory sensation,” in Handbook of Physiology, N. S. Cherniack and J. G. Widdicombe, Eds., vol. 2 of Control of Breathing, I, Section 3: The Respiratory System, pp. 449–474, American Physiological Society, Bethesda, Md, USA, 1986.
[24]
N. Wolcove, M. D. Altose, S. G. Kelson, P. G. Kondapalli, and N. S. Cherniack, “Perception of changes in breathing in normal human subjects,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 78–83, 1981.
[25]
A. L. Gilchrist and A. Radonji?, “Anchoring of lightness values by relative luminance and relative area,” Journal of Vision, vol. 9, no. 9, article 13, 10 pages, 2009.
[26]
G. Canévet and B. Scharf, “The loudness of sounds that increase and decrease continuously in level,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 88, no. 5, pp. 2136–2142, 1990.
[27]
M. Epstein and M. Florentine, “Loudness of brief tones measured by magnitude estimation and loudness matching,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 119, no. 4, pp. 1943–1945, 2006.
[28]
R. P. Hellman and J. J. Zwislocki, “Loudness function of a 1000-cps tone in the presence of a masking noise,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 36, pp. 1618–1627, 1964.
[29]
G. A. Gescheider and B. A. Hughson, “Stimulus context and absolute magnitude estimation: a study of individual differences,” Perception and Psychophysics, vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 45–57, 1991.
[30]
L. L. Andersen, C. H. Andersen, O. S. Mortensen, O. M. Poulsen, I. B. T. Bj?rnlund, and M. K. Zebis, “Muscle activation and perceived loading during rehabilitation exercises: comparison of dumbbells and elastic resistance,” Physical Therapy, vol. 90, no. 4, pp. 538–549, 2010.
[31]
D. F. Cooper, G. Grimby, D. A. Jones, and R. H. T. Edwards, “Perception of effort in isometric and dynamic muscular contraction,” European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, vol. 41, no. 3, pp. 173–180, 1979.
[32]
D. M. Pincivero, A. J. Coelho, and R. M. Campy, “Perceived exertion and maximal quadriceps femoris muscle strength during dynamic knee extension exercise in young adult males and females,” European Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 89, no. 2, pp. 150–156, 2003.
[33]
American Thoracic Society, “Standards for the diagnosis and care of patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) and asthma,” American Review of Respiratory Disease, vol. 136, no. 1, pp. 225–244, 1987.
[34]
A. A. Collins and G. A. Gescheider, “The measurement of loudness in individual children and adults by absolute magnitude estimation and cross-modality matching,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 85, no. 5, pp. 2012–2021, 1989.