6 Zhou X, Chen Y, Chen C, et al. Chinese kindergartners' automatic processing of numerical magnitude in stroop-like tasks. Mem Cognit, 2007, 35: 464-470
[6]
7 Longo M R, Lourenco S F. Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space. Brain Cognition, 2010, 72: 362-367
[7]
10 Merritt D J, Casasanto D, Brannon E M. Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humans. Cognition, 2010, 117: 191-202
[8]
15 Oliveri M, Vicario C M, Salerno S, et al. Perceiving numbers alters time perception. Neurosci Lett, 2008, 438: 308-311
[9]
17 Hinton S C, Harrington D L, Binder J R, et al. Neural systems supporting timing and chronometric counting: An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Res, 2004, 21: 183-192
[10]
18 Buhusi C V, Meck W H. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci, 2005, 6: 755-765
[11]
22 Dehaene S, Bossini S, Giraux P. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J Exp Psychol Gen, 1993, 122: 371-396
[12]
25 Fischer M H, Castel A D, Dodd M D, et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nat Neurosci, 2003, 6: 555-556
[13]
28 Jewell G, McCourt M E. Pseudoneglect: A review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 2000, 38: 93-110
[14]
29 G?bel S M, Calabria M, Farnè A, et al. Parietal rtms distorts the mental number line: Simulating “spatial” neglect in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44: 860-868
[15]
31 Lourenco S F, Longo M R. Multiple spatial representations of number: Evidence for co-existing compressive and linear scales. Exp Brain Res, 2009, 193: 151-156
[16]
32 Longo M R, Lourenco S F. Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 2007, 45: 1400-1406
[17]
36 Lourenco S F, Longo M R. The plasticity of near space: Evidence for contraction. Cognition, 2009, 112: 451-456
[18]
37 Zorzi M, Priftis K, Umilta C. Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 2002, 417: 138-139
[19]
43 de Long A J. Phenomenological space-time: Towards an experiential relativity. Science, 1981, 213: 681-683
[20]
44 Srinivasan M, Carey S. The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time. Cognition, 2010, 116: 217-241
[21]
45 Vicario C M, Pecoraro P, Turriziani P, et al. Relativistic compression and expansion of experiential time in the left and right space. PLoS One, 2008, 3: e1716
[22]
46 Ishihara M, Keller P E, Rossetti Y, et al. Horizontal spatial representations of time: Evidence for the stearc effect. Cortex, 2008, 44: 454-461
[23]
48 Z?ch P, Brugger P. Subjective time in near and far representational space. Cogn Behav Neurol, 2008, 21: 8-13
[24]
49 Boroditsky L. Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 2000, 75: 1-28
[25]
50 Casasanto D, Fotakopoulou O, Boroditsky L. Space and time in the child's mind: Evidence for a cross-dimensional asymmetry. Cogn Sci, 2010, 34: 387-405
[26]
51 Walsh V. A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends Cogn Sci, 2003, 7: 483-488
[27]
53 Castelli F, Glaser D E, Butterworth B. Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: A functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 2006, 103: 4693-4698
[28]
54 Fias W, Lammertyn J, Reynvoet B, et al. Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J Cogn Neurosci, 2003, 15: 47-56
[29]
55 Cohen-Kadosh R, Lammertyn J, Izard V. Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental, and comparative studies of magnitude representation. Prog Neurobiol, 2008, 84: 132-147
[30]
56 Cohen-Kadosh R, Cohen-Kadosh K, Linden D E J, et al. The brain locus of interaction between number and size: A combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potential study. J Cogn Neurosci, 2007, 19: 957-970
[31]
58 Roberts S, Church R M. Control of an internal clock. J Exp Psychol Anim B, 1978, 4: 318-337
66 Casasanto D, Jasmin K. Good and bad in the hands of politicians: Spontaneous gestures during positive and negative speech. PLoS ONE, 2010, 5: e11805
[34]
3 Dehaene S, Brannon E M. Space, time, and number: A kantian research program. Trends Cogn Sci, 2010, 14: 517-519
[35]
8 Lu A, Hodges B, Zhang J, et al. Contextual effects on number-time interaction. Cognition, 2009, 113: 117-122
[36]
9 Xuan B, Chen X C, He S, et al. Numerical magnitude modulates temporal comparison: An ERP study. Brain Res, 2009, 1269: 135-142
[37]
11 Zhou X, Chen C, Dong Q, et al. Event-related potentials of single-digit addition, subtraction, and multiplication. Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44: 2500-2507
14 Meck W H, Church R M. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim B, 1983, 9: 320-334
[41]
16 Dormal V, Seron X, Pesenti M. Numerosityduration interference: A stroop experiment. Acta Psychol, 2006, 121: 109-124
[42]
19 Cappelletti M, Freeman E D, Cipolotti L. Dissociations and interactions between time, numerosity and space processing. Neuropsychologia, 2009, 47: 2732-2748
[43]
20 Macar F, Grondin S, Casini L. Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Mem Cognit, 1994, 22: 673-686
[44]
21 Brown S W. Attentional resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Percept Psychophys, 1997, 59: 1118-1140
[45]
23 Pinel P, Piazza M, Le Bihan D, et al. Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgments. Neuron, 2004, 41: 983-993
[46]
24 Doricchi F, Guariglia P, Gasparini M, et al. Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nat Neurosci, 2005, 8: 1663-1665
27 Bowers D, Heilman K. Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 1980, 18: 491-498
[49]
30 Loftus A M, Nicholls M E, Mattingley J B, et al. Pseudoneglect for the bisection of mental number lines. Q J Exp Psychol, 2009, 62: 925-945
[50]
33 Loetscher T, Schwarz U, Schubiger M, et al. Head turns bias the brain's internal random generator. Curr Biol, 2008, 18: R60-R62
[51]
34 Longo M R, Lourenco S F. On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space. Neuropsychologia, 2006, 44: 977-981
[52]
35 Longo M R, Lourenco S F. Space perception and body morphology: Extent of near space scales with arm length. Exp Brain Res, 2007, 177: 285-290
[53]
38 Pia L, Corazzini L L, Folegatti A, et al. Mental number line disruption in a right-neglect patient after a left-hemisphere stroke. Brain Cognition, 2009, 69: 81-88
[54]
39 Bjoertomt O, Cowey A, Walsh V. Near space functioning of the human angular and supramarginal gyri. J Neuropsychol, 2009, 3: 31-43
[55]
40 Loetscher T, Brugger P. Exploring number space by random digit generation. Exp Brain Res, 2007, 180: 655-665
[56]
41 Basso G, Nichelli P, Frassinetti F, et al. Time perception in a neglected space. Neuroreport, 1996, 7: 2111-2114
[57]
42 Casasanto D, Boroditsky L. Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 2008, 106: 579-593
[58]
47 Vallesi A, Binns M A, Shallice T. An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition, 2008, 107: 501-527
[59]
52 Moyer R S, Landauer T K. Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 1967, 215: 1519-1520
[60]
57 Breukelaar J W C, Dalrymple-Alford J C. Timing ability and numerical competence in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim B, 1998, 24: 84-97
[61]
59 Bueti D, Walsh V. The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philos Trans R Soc B-Biol Sci, 2009, 364: 1831-1840
[62]
60 Burr D C, Ross J, Binda P, et al. Saccades compress space, time and number. Trends Cogn Sci, 2010, 14: 528-533
[63]
61 Cantlon J F, Platt M L, Brannon E M. Beyond the number domain. Trends Cogn Sci, 2009, 13: 83-91