Autism is a common and often highly debilitating neurodevelopmental condition, whose core behavioral features are believed to be rooted in disrupted neurocognitive processes, including especially “executive function.” Researchers have predominantly focused upon understanding the putative causal relationship between difficulties in EF and autistic symptomatology. This paper suggests, however, that the effects of individual differences in EF should be more far-reaching, playing a significant part in the real-life outcomes of individuals with autism, including their social competence, everyday adaptive behavior, and academic achievement. It further considers the nature of the EF-outcome relationship, including the possible determinants of individual differences in EF, and makes several recommendations for future research. 1. Introduction Autism spectrum conditions (hereafter, “autism”) are a set of common, lifelong neurodevelopmental conditions that involve substantial heterogeneity at numerous levels, including etiology, neurobiology, cognition, and especially behavior. Long-term follow-up studies show that the developmental outcomes of autistic individuals are highly variable, even for individuals at the more intellectually able end of the autism spectrum. While some individuals go on to live independently and obtain qualifications, the majority fail to achieve independence, to attain full-time employment, or to enjoy friendships [1–5] (though see [6]). Explaining this variability is of critical import: to discover why developments take place in some areas and not in others, and especially in some individuals and not in others. Researchers have little understanding of the factors underpinning this heterogeneity, due in part to a dearth of longitudinal studies tracing the development of autism and to the preponderance of studies using a case-control design, focusing on group rather than on individual differences. This paper aims to address this limitation. It identifies one potential source of this variability, namely, autistic children’s emerging “executive function” (EF), those higher-order processes, closely associated with the prefrontal cortex, which are necessary for regulating and controlling behavior (see Table 1). Specifically, it suggests that individual differences in the growth trajectories of autistic children’s EF skills could explain some of the variability in children’s functional outcomes, including their social awareness, real-life adaptive behavior, and readiness to learn in school, both in the shorter term and in the longer term. Table
References
[1]
P. Howlin, S. Goode, J. Hutton, and M. Rutter, “Adult outcome for children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 212–229, 2004.
[2]
P. Howlin, L. Mawhood, and M. Rutter, “Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 41, no. 5, pp. 561–578, 2000.
[3]
L. Kanner, Childhood Psychosis: Initial Studies and New Insights, Winston/Wiley, New York, NY, USA, 1973.
[4]
M. Sigman and E. Ruskin, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial no. 256, vol. 4, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Ill, USA, 1999, Social competence in children with autism, Down syndrome and other developmental delays: a longitudinal study.
[5]
P. Szatmari, G. Bartolucci, R. Bremner, S. Bond, and S. Rich, “A follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 213–225, 1989.
[6]
M. A. Farley, W. M. McMahon, E. Fombonne et al., “Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and average or near-average cognitive abilities,” Autism Research, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 109–118, 2009.
[7]
C. Hughes and J. Russell, “Autistic children’s difficulty with mental disengagement from an object: its implications for theories of autism,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 498–510, 1993.
[8]
S. Ozonoff, B. F. Pennington, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: relationship to theory of mind,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 32, no. 7, pp. 1081–1105, 1991.
[9]
C. Hughes, J. Russell, and T. W. Robbins, “Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 477–492, 1994.
[10]
C. Hughes, M. Leboyer, and M. Bouvard, “Executive function in parents of children with autism,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 209–220, 1997.
[11]
C. Hughes, M. H. Plumet, and M. Leboyer, “Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 40, no. 5, pp. 705–718, 1999.
[12]
E. L. Hill, “Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism,” Developmental Review, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 189–233, 2004.
[13]
N. Russo, T. Flanagan, G. Iarocci, D. Berringer, P. D. Zelazo, and J. A. Burack, “Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 65, no. 1, pp. 77–86, 2007.
[14]
H. M. Geurts, S. Verté, J. Oosterlaan, H. Roeyers, and J. A. Sergeant, “How specific are executive functioning deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and autism?” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 4, pp. 836–854, 2004.
[15]
S. Ozonoff and J. Jensen, “Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 171–177, 1999.
[16]
H. M. Geurts, B. Corbett, and M. Solomon, “The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 74–82, 2009.
[17]
M. Liss, D. Fein, D. Allen et al., “Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 261–270, 2001.
[18]
E. Pellicano, “Links between theory of mind and executive function in young children with autism: clues to developmental primacy,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 974–990, 2007.
[19]
E. Pellicano, “The development of core cognitive skills in Autism: a 3-year prospective study,” Child Development, vol. 81, no. 5, pp. 1400–1416, 2010.
[20]
E. M. Griffith, B. F. Pennington, E. A. Wehner, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive functions in young children with autism,” Child Development, vol. 70, no. 4, pp. 817–832, 1999.
[21]
B. E. Yerys, S. L. Hepburn, B. F. Pennington, and S. J. Rogers, “Executive function in preschoolers with autism: evidence consistent with a secondary deficit,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 6, pp. 1068–1079, 2007.
[22]
G. Dawson, J. Munson, A. Estes et al., “Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay,” Child Development, vol. 73, no. 2, pp. 345–358, 2002.
[23]
F. Happé, A. Ronald, and R. Plomin, “Time to give up on a single explanation for autism,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 10, pp. 1218–1220, 2006.
[24]
S. M. Carlson and L. J. Moses, “Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind,” Child Development, vol. 72, no. 4, pp. 1032–1053, 2001.
[25]
D. Frye, P. D. Zelazo, and T. Palfai, “Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning,” Cognitive Development, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 483–527, 1995.
[26]
C. Hughes, “Executive function in preschoolers: links with theory of mind and verbal ability,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 233–253, 1998.
[27]
C. Hughes, “Finding your marbles: does preschoolers' strategic behavior predict later understanding of mind?” Developmental Psychology, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 1326–1339, 1998.
[28]
S. M. Carlson, D. J. Mandell, and L. Williams, “Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 40, no. 6, pp. 1105–1122, 2004.
[29]
C. Hughes and R. Ensor, “Executive function and theory of mind: predictive relations from ages 2 to 4,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 6, pp. 1447–1459, 2007.
[30]
L. J. Moses, “Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development,” Child Development, vol. 72, no. 3, pp. 688–690, 2001.
[31]
J. Russell, “How executive disorders can bring about an adequate theory of mind,” in Autism as an Executive Disorder, J. Russell, Ed., pp. 256–304, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 1997.
[32]
C. Blair, “School readiness: integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry,” American Psychologist, vol. 57, no. 2, pp. 111–127, 2002.
[33]
M. M. McClelland, C. E. Cameron, C. M. Connor, C. L. Farris, A. M. Jewkes, and F. J. Morrison, “Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary and maths skills,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 43, no. 4, pp. 947–959, 2007.
[34]
C. Blair and R. P. Razza, “Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten,” Child Development, vol. 78, no. 2, pp. 647–663, 2007.
[35]
R. Bull and G. Scerif, “Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: inhibition, switching, and working memory,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 273–293, 2001.
[36]
H. L. St Clair-Thompson and S. E. Gathercole, “Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 745–759, 2006.
[37]
A. Diamond, “Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, D. Stuss and R. Knight, Eds., pp. 466–503, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2002.
[38]
A. Diamond, W. S. Barnett, J. Thomas, and S. Munro, “The early years: preschool program improves cognitive control,” Science, vol. 318, no. 5855, pp. 1387–1388, 2007.
[39]
J. Holmes, S. E. Gathercole, and D. L. Dunning, “Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children,” Developmental Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. F9–F15, 2009.
[40]
G. Dawson, A. N. Meltzoff, J. Osterling, and J. Rinaldi, “Neuropsychological correlates of early symptoms of autism,” Child Development, vol. 69, no. 5, pp. 1276–1285, 1998.
[41]
L. Gilotty, L. Kenworthy, D. O. Black, A. E. Wagner, and L. Sirian, “Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders,” Child Neuropsychology, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 241–248, 2002.
[42]
F. Happé, R. Booth, R. Charlton, and C. Hughes, “Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 25–39, 2006.
[43]
H. J. C. Berger, F. H. T. M. Aerts, K. P. M. Van Spaendonck, A. R. Cools, and J. P. Teunisse, “Central coherence and cognitive shifting in relation to social improvement in high-functioning young adults with autism,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 502–511, 2003.
[44]
D. Wechsler, Wechsler Scales of Intelligence, The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 4th edition, 2003.
[45]
B. Milner, “Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man,” British Medical Bulletin, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 272–277, 1971.
[46]
C. Hughes, “Control of action and thought: normal development and dysfunction in autism: a research note,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 37, no. 2, pp. 229–236, 1996.
[47]
A. R. Luria, K. H. Pribram, and E. D. Homskaya, “An experimental analysis of the behavioral disturbance produced by a left frontal arachnoidal endothelioma (meningioma),” Neuropsychologia, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 257–280, 1964.
[48]
P. D. Zelazo, “The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children,” Nature Protocols, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 297–301, 2006.
[49]
T. Shallice, “Specific impairments of planning,” Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, vol. 298, no. 1089, pp. 199–209, 1982.
[50]
S. Baron-Cohen, A. M. Leslie, and U. Frith, “Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ?” Cognition, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 37–46, 1985.
[51]
H. Wimmer and J. Perner, “Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception,” Cognition, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 103–128, 1983.
[52]
P. Mundy, A. Hogan, and P. Doehring, A Preliminary Manual for the Abridged Early Social-Communication Scales, University of Miami, Coral Gables, Fla, USA, 1996, http://www.ucdmc.ucdavis.edu/mindinstitute/ourteam/faculty_staff/escs.pdf.
[53]
S. Sparrow, D. Cicchetti, and D. Balla, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Pearson Assessment, Minneapolis, Minn, USA, 2nd edition, 2005.
[54]
E. Pellicano, “Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 530–544, 2010.
[55]
J. Duncan, “An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 11, pp. 820–829, 2001.
[56]
J. Duncan and A. M. Owen, “Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 23, no. 10, pp. 475–483, 2000.
[57]
T. Shallice and P. W. Burgess, “Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man,” Brain, vol. 114, no. 2, pp. 727–741, 1991.
[58]
Y. Munakata, “Graded representations in behavioral dissociations,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 5, no. 7, pp. 309–315, 2001.
[59]
P. D. Zelazo, U. Müller, and D. Frye, “The development of executive function in early childhood,” Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 1–27, 2003.
[60]
A. Diamond, “The early development of executive functions,” in Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of Change, E. Bialystock and F. I. M. Craik, Eds., pp. 70–95, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2006.
[61]
N. Garon, S. E. Bryson, and I. M. Smith, “Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 31–60, 2008.
[62]
A. Miyake, N. P. Friedman, M. J. Emerson, A. H. Witzki, A. Howerter, and T. D. Wager, “The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 49–100, 2000.
[63]
M. Huizinga, C. V. Dolan, and M. W. van der Molen, “Age-related change in executive function: developmental trends and a latent variable analysis,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 44, no. 11, pp. 2017–2036, 2006.
[64]
J. E. Lehto, P. Juuj?rvi, L. Kooistra, and L. Pulkkinen, “Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children,” British Journal of Developmental Psychology, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 59–80, 2003.
[65]
S. A. Wiebe, K. A. Espy, and D. Charak, “Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: i. Latent structure,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 575–587, 2008.
[66]
S. A. Wiebe, T. Sheffield, J. M. Nelson, C. A. C. Clark, N. Chevalier, and K. A. Espy, “The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 436–452, 2011.
[67]
A. Karmiloff-Smith, “Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: rethinking the study of developmental disorders,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 45, no. 1, pp. 56–63, 2009.
[68]
K. J. Towgood, J. D. I. Meuwese, S. J. Gilbert, M. S. Turner, and P. W. Burgess, “Advantages of the multiple case series approach to the study of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 47, no. 13, pp. 2981–2988, 2009.
[69]
P. R. Huttenlocher, Neural Plasticity: The Effects of the Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2002.
[70]
C. A. Nelson, K. M. Thomas, and M. de Haan, Neuroscience and Cognitive Development: The Role of Experience and the Developing Brain, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006.
[71]
W. T. Greenough, J. E. Black, and C. S. Wallace, “Experience and brain development,” Child Development, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 539–559, 1987.
[72]
M. I. Posner and M. K. Rothbart, “Developing mechanisms of self-regulation,” Development and Psychopathology, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 427–441, 2000.
[73]
M. R. Rueda, J. Fan, B. D. McCandliss et al., “Development of attentional networks in childhood,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 42, no. 8, pp. 1029–1040, 2004.
[74]
M. R. Rueda, M. I. Posner, and M. K. Rothbart, “The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 573–594, 2005.
[75]
J. A. Burack, “Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 103, no. 3, pp. 535–543, 1994.
[76]
R. Landry and S. E. Bryson, “Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with austism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 6, pp. 1115–1122, 2004.
[77]
T. A. Mann and P. Walker, “Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 274–284, 2003.
[78]
A. Baddeley, Working Memory, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 1986.
[79]
A. R. Luria, The Role of Speech in the Regulation of Normal and Abnormal Behavior, Pergamon Press, London, UK, 1961.
[80]
A. R. Luria, Higher Cortical Functions in Man, Basic Books, New York, NY, USA, 1966.
[81]
L. Vygotsky, Thought and Language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.), MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1962.
[82]
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR), American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, 2000.
[83]
J. Russell, Agency: Its Role in Mental Development, Erlbaum, Hove, UK, 1996.
[84]
R. M. Joseph, S. D. Steele, E. Meyer, and H. Tager-Flusberg, “Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory?” Neuropsychologia, vol. 43, no. 10, pp. 1400–1411, 2005.
[85]
G. L. Wallace, J. A. Silvers, A. Martin, and L. E. Kenworthy, “Brief report: further evidence for inner speech deficits in Autism spectrum disorders,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 39, no. 12, pp. 1735–1739, 2009.
[86]
A. J. O. Whitehouse, M. T. Maybery, and K. Durkin, “Inner speech impairments in autism,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 47, no. 8, pp. 857–865, 2006.
[87]
A. R. Luria, Language and Cognition, John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, USA, 1981.
[88]
L. Vygotsky, Mind and Society: The Development of Higher Mental Processes, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1978.
[89]
K. G. Noble, M. F. Norman, and M. J. Farah, “Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children,” Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 74–87, 2005.
[90]
A. Bernier, S. M. Carlson, and N. Whipple, “From external regulation to self-regulation: early parenting precursors of young children's executive functioning,” Child Development, vol. 81, no. 1, pp. 326–339, 2010.
[91]
C. H. Hughes and R. A. Ensor, “How do families help or hinder the emergence of early executive function?” New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, vol. 2009, no. 123, pp. 35–50, 2009.
[92]
B. L. Rhoades, M. T. Greenberg, S. T. Lanza, and C. Blair, “Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: contribution of a person-centered perspective,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 638–662, 2011.
[93]
A. H. Rutgers, M. J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. H. van IJzendoorn, and I. A. van Berckelaer-Onnes, “Autism and attachment: a meta-analytic review,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 45, no. 6, pp. 1123–1134, 2004.
[94]
F. Knott, C. Lewis, and T. Williams, “Sibling interaction of children with autism: development over 12 months,” Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, vol. 37, no. 10, pp. 1987–1995, 2007.