This fMRI study investigates the effect of melody on aesthetic experience in listeners na?ve to formal musical knowledge. Using simple melodic lines, whose syntactic structure was manipulated, we created systematic acoustic dissonance. Two stimulus categories were created: canonical (syntactically “correct,” in the Western culture) and modified (made of an altered version of the canonical melodies). The stimuli were presented under two tasks: listening and aesthetic judgment. Data were analyzed as a function of stimulus structure (canonical and modified) and stimulus aesthetics, as appraised by each participant during scanning. The critical contrast modified versus canonical stimuli produced enhanced activation of deep temporal regions, including the parahippocampus, suggesting that melody manipulation induced feelings of unpleasantness in the listeners. This was supported by our behavioral data indicating decreased aesthetic preference for the modified melodies. Medial temporal activation could also have been evoked by stimulus structural novelty determining increased memory load for the modified stimuli. The analysis of melodies judged as beautiful revealed that aesthetic judgment of simple melodies relied on a fine-structural analysis of the stimuli subserved by a left frontal activation and, possibly, on meaning attribution at the charge of right superior temporal sulcus for increasingly pleasurable stimuli. 1. Introduction Music is simultaneously art and science: it allows the artist to express his/her inner world through sounds, which are linked one to another by stringent rules that are strongly influenced by culture. These rules represent hallmarks that, on one side, constrain the composer’s freedom to choose associations and successions of sounds and, on the other, offer a context, within which all elements gain a meaning. Traditionally, there has been a strong tendency to emphasize the dominance of compositional structures in outlining the aesthetic character of a musical piece. In the present study, we investigated this relationship by exploring the aesthetics of melody, that is, the capacity of simple musical structures to evoke an aesthetic experience in listeners na?ve to formal musical knowledge. Music is made of rules that govern the relation between notes and of a dynamic dimension that defines its tempo and rhythm. As far as the succession of sounds is concerned, the founding rules of a musical piece are also referred to as syntactic rules (this denomination implicitly underlines the similarities between music and language). Music
References
[1]
J. L. Chen, R. J. Zatorre, and V. B. Penhune, “Interactions between auditory and dorsal premotor cortex during synchronization to musical rhythms,” NeuroImage, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 1771–1781, 2006.
[2]
J. L. Chen, V. B. Penhune, and R. J. Zatorre, “Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 18, no. 12, pp. 2844–2854, 2008.
[3]
P. Janata and S. T. Grafton, “Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 6, no. 7, pp. 682–687, 2003.
[4]
B. Maess, S. Koelsch, T. C. Gunter, and A. D. Friederici, “Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 4, no. 5, pp. 540–545, 2001.
[5]
B. Tillmann, S. Koelsch, N. Escoffier et al., “Cognitive priming in sung and instrumental music: activation of inferior frontal cortex,” NeuroImage, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 1771–1782, 2006.
[6]
B. Tillmann, S. Koelsch, N. Escoffier et al., “Cognitive priming in sung and instrumental music: activation of inferior frontal cortex,” NeuroImage, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 1771–1782, 2006.
[7]
D. J. Levitin and V. Menon, “The neural locus of temporal structure and expectancies in music: evidence from functional neuroimaging at 3 tesla,” Music Perception, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 563–575, 2005.
[8]
D. Perani, M. C. Saccuman, P. Scifo et al., “Functional specializations for music processing in the human newborn brain,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 107, no. 10, pp. 4758–4763, 2010.
[9]
A. J. Blood and R. J. Zatorre, “Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 98, no. 20, pp. 11818–11823, 2001.
[10]
S. Koelsch, T. Fritz, D. Y. V. Cramon, K. Müller, and A. D. Friederici, “Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 27, no. 3, pp. 239–250, 2006.
[11]
S. Koelsch, S. Skouras, T. Fritz et al., “The roles of superficial amygdala and auditory cortex in music-evoked fear and joy,” NeuroImage, vol. 31, pp. 1771–1782, 2013.
[12]
K. J. Worsley and K. J. Friston, “Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited—again,” NeuroImage, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 173–181, 1995.
[13]
K. J. Friston, “Bayesian estimation of dynamical systems: an application to fMRI,” NeuroImage, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 513–530, 2002.
[14]
J. L. R. Andersson, C. Hutton, J. Ashburner, R. Turner, and K. Friston, “Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series,” NeuroImage, vol. 13, no. 5, pp. 903–919, 2001.
[15]
D. L. Collins, P. Neelin, T. M. Peters, and A. C. Evans, “Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space,” Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 192–205, 1994.
[16]
C. J. Limb, “Structural and functional neural correlates of music perception,” Anatomical Record Part A: Discoveries in Molecular, Cellular, and Evolutionary Biology, vol. 288, no. 4, pp. 435–446, 2006.
[17]
R. J. Zatorre, A. C. Evans, and E. Meyer, “Neural mechanisms underlying melodic perception and memory for pitch,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 14, no. 4, pp. 1908–1919, 1994.
[18]
A. R. Halpern and R. J. Zatorre, “When that tune runs through your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 9, no. 7, pp. 697–704, 1999.
[19]
I. Peretz and R. J. Zatorre, “Brain organization for music processing,” Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 56, pp. 89–114, 2005.
[20]
A. S. Bregman, Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound, The MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1990.
[21]
T. D. Griffiths and J. D. Warren, “The planum temporale as a computational hub,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 25, no. 7, pp. 348–353, 2002.
[22]
C. Liégeois-Chauvel, I. Peretz, M. Baba?, V. Laguitton, and P. Chauvel, “Contribution of different cortical areas in the temporal lobes to music processing,” Brain, vol. 121, no. 10, pp. 1853–1867, 1998.
[23]
R. D. Patterson, S. Uppenkamp, I. S. Johnsrude, and T. D. Griffiths, “The processing of temporal pitch and melody information in auditory cortex,” Neuron, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 767–776, 2002.
[24]
J. R. Augustine, “Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 229–244, 1996.
[25]
D.-E. Bamiou, F. E. Musiek, and L. M. Luxon, “The insula (Island of Reil) and its role in auditory processing: literature review,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 143–154, 2003.
[26]
A. J. Blood, R. J. Zatorre, P. Bermudez, and A. C. Evans, “Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 382–387, 1999.
[27]
N. Gosselin, S. Samson, R. Adolphs et al., “Emotional responses to unpleasant music correlates with damage to the parahippocampal cortex,” Brain, vol. 129, no. 10, pp. 2585–2592, 2006.
[28]
S.-H. Wang and R. G. M. Morris, “Hippocampal-neocortical interactions in memory formation, consolidation, and reconsolidation,” Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 61, pp. 49–79, 2010.
[29]
N. M. van Strien, N. L. M. Cappaert, and M. P. Witter, “The anatomy of memory: an interactive overview of the parahippocampal- hippocampal network,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 272–282, 2009.
[30]
G. Bottini, R. Corcoran, R. Sterzi, et al., “The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language: a positron emission tomography activation study,” Brain, vol. 117, no. 6, pp. 1241–1253, 1994.
[31]
M. Sotillo, L. Carretié, J. A. Hinojosa et al., “Neural activity associated with metaphor comprehension: spatial analysis,” Neuroscience Letters, vol. 373, no. 1, pp. 5–9, 2005.
[32]
Y. Harpaz, Y. Levkovitz, and M. Lavidor, “Lexical ambiguity resolution in Wernicke's area and its right homologue,” Cortex, vol. 45, no. 9, pp. 1097–1103, 2009.
[33]
M. Jung-Beeman, “Bilateral brain processes for comprehending natural language,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 9, no. 11, pp. 512–518, 2005.
[34]
S. Koelsch, E. Kasper, D. Sammler, K. Schulze, T. Gunter, and A. D. Friederici, “Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 302–307, 2004.
[35]
S. Koelsch, T. Fritz, K. Schulze, D. Alsop, and G. Schlaug, “Adults and children processing music: an fMRI study,” NeuroImage, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 1068–1076, 2005.
[36]
J. Daltrozzo and D. Sch?n, “Is conceptual processing in music automatic? An electrophysiological approach,” Brain Research, vol. 1270, pp. 88–94, 2009.
[37]
N. Steinbeis and S. Koelsch, “Shared neural resources between music and language indicate semantic processing of musical tension-resolution patterns,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 1169–1178, 2008.
[38]
M. Himmelbach, M. Erb, and H.-O. Karnath, “Exploring the visual world: the neural substrate of spatial orienting,” NeuroImage, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 1747–1759, 2006.
[39]
J. Downar, A. P. Crawley, D. J. Mikulis, and K. D. Davis, “A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities,” Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 87, no. 1, pp. 615–620, 2002.
[40]
H.-O. Karnath, “New insights into the functions of the superior temporal cortex,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 8, pp. 568–576, 2001.
[41]
S. Koelsch, “Towards a neural basis of music-evoked emotions,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 131–137, 2010.
[42]
D. D. Cinzia and G. Vittorio, “Neuroaesthetics: a review,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol. 19, no. 6, pp. 682–687, 2009.
[43]
D. Sammler, M. Grigutsch, T. Fritz, and S. Koelsch, “Music and emotion: electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant music,” Psychophysiology, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 293–304, 2007.