Parkinson's disease (PD) and Tourette's syndrome (TS) lead to important motor disorders among patients such as possible facial amimia in PD and tics in Tourette's syndrome. Under the grounded cognition framework that shows the importance of motor embodiment in emotional feeling (Niedenthal, 2007), both types of pathology with motor symptoms should be sufficient to induce potential impairments for these patients when recognizing emotional facial expressions (EFE). In this opinion paper, we describe a theoretical framework that assumes potential emotional disorders in Parkinson's disease and Tourette's syndrome based on motor disorders characterizing these two pathologies. We also review different methodological barriers in previous experimental designs that could enable the identification of emotional facial expressions despite emotional disorders in PD and TS. 1. Grounded Cognition Theory A prominent view of cognitive and emotional processes assumes that conceptual knowledge emerges from bottom-up perceptual processes to conceptual and a modal symbolic systems [1]. An alternative view has been proposed under the theoretical framework of theories of grounded cognition [2–4] suggesting that associative, limbic, and sensorimotor functional components are automatically reactivated for access to specific conceptual knowledge. This theoretical view assumes that disturbing motor processing can induce recognition impairment at the perceptual level. For instance, Strack et al. [5] have shown that participants holding a pen between their lips to inhibit smiling, or holding the pen between their teeth to facilitate smiling (Figure 1), produced significant emotional modulation with respect to the funniness of cartoons (depending on the group of inhibited facial muscles). In what follows, we argue that consistent with the grounded cognition theory, motor disorders characterizing different psychopathologies (Parkinson’s disease and Tourette’s syndrome) could be sufficient to induce disturbances in emotion processing. Figure 1: Experimental procedure used by Strack et al. [ 5]. Different neural structures have been identified as playing an important role in grounded cognition. For example, these type of links between motor systems and action understanding had previously been developed in the literature on mirror neurons, first shown in monkeys [6, 7] and more recently among humans where premotor and parietal areas were identified as important neural structures for mirror neuron system [6–8]. Cortical mirror neurons were interpreted as automatic neural processes
References
[1]
G. H. Bower, “Mood and memory,” American Psychologist, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 129–148, 1981.
[2]
L. W. Barsalou, “Perceptual symbol systems,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 577–609, 1999.
[3]
P. M. Niedenthal, “Embodying emotion,” Science, vol. 316, no. 5827, pp. 1002–1005, 2007.
[4]
P. M. Niedenthal, M. Mermillod, M. Maringer, and U. Hess, “The future of SIMS: who embodies which smile and when?” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol. 33, no. 6, pp. 464–480, 2010.
[5]
F. Strack, L. L. Martin, and S. Stepper, “Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 54, no. 5, pp. 768–777, 1988.
[6]
V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti, “Action recognition in the premotor cortex,” Brain, vol. 119, no. 2, pp. 593–609, 1996.
[7]
G. Rizzolatti, L. Fadiga, V. Gallese, and L. Fogassi, “Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 131–141, 1996.
[8]
J. Decety and J. Grèzes, “Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 3, no. 5, pp. 172–178, 1999.
[9]
B. Wicker, C. Keysers, J. Plailly, J. P. Royet, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti, “Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust,” Neuron, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 655–664, 2003.
[10]
R. Adolphs, H. Damasio, D. Tranel, and A. R. Damasio, “Cortical systems for the recognition of emotion in facial expressions,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 16, no. 23, pp. 7678–7687, 1996.
[11]
R. Adolphs, H. Damasio, D. Tranel, G. Cooper, and A. R. Damasio, “A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 20, no. 7, pp. 2683–2690, 2000.
[12]
R. Adolphs, “Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms,” Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 21–62, 2002.
[13]
J. Yelnik, E. Bardinet, D. Dormont et al., “A three-dimensional, histological and deformable atlas of the human basal ganglia. I. Atlas construction based on immunohistochemical and MRI data,” NeuroImage, vol. 34, no. 2, pp. 618–638, 2007.
[14]
P. K. Doshi, N. Chhaya, and M. H. Bhatt, “Depression leading to attempted suicide after bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation for Parkinson's disease,” Movement Disorders, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 1084–1085, 2002.
[15]
J. L. Houeto, V. Mesnage, L. Mallet, et al., “Behavioral disorders, Parkinson’s disease and subthalamic nucleus,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 72, pp. 701–707, 2002.
[16]
M. Ulla, S. Thobois, J. J. Lemaire et al., “Manic behaviour induced by deep-brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease: evidence of substantia nigra implication?” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 77, no. 12, pp. 1363–1366, 2006.
[17]
A. Parent, “Extrinsic connections of the basal ganglia,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 13, no. 7, pp. 254–258, 1990.
[18]
P. Derost, L. Ouchchane, D. Morand, et al., “Is DBS-STN appropriate to manage severe Parkinson’s disease in an elderly population?” Neurology, vol. 68, no. 17, pp. 1345–1355, 2007.
[19]
P. Limousin, P. Krack, P. Pollak et al., “Electrical Stimulation of the Subthalamic Nucleus in Advanced Parkinson's Disease,” The New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 339, pp. 1105–1111, 1998.
[20]
I. Biseul, P. Sauleau, C. Haegelen et al., “Fear recognition is impaired by subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson's disease,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 43, no. 7, pp. 1054–1059, 2005.
[21]
K. Dujardin, S. Blairy, L. Defebvre et al., “Deficits in decoding emotional facial expressions in Parkinson's disease,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 239–250, 2004.
[22]
U. Dimberg, “Facial electromyography and emotional reactions,” Psychophysiology, vol. 27, no. 5, pp. 481–494, 1990.
[23]
U. Hess and S. Blairy, “Facial mimicry and emotional contagion to dynamic emotional facial expressions and their influence on decoding accuracy,” International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 40, no. 2, pp. 129–141, 2001.
[24]
J. W. Mink, “Basal ganglia dysfunction in Tourette's syndrome: a new hypothesis,” Pediatric Neurology, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 190–198, 2001.
[25]
D. C. Osmon and J. M. Smerz, “Neuropsychological evaluation in the diagnosis and treatment of Tourette's syndrome,” Behavior Modification, vol. 29, no. 5, pp. 746–783, 2005.
[26]
R. Sprengelmeyer, A. W. Young, I. Pundt et al., “Disgust implicated in obsessive-compulsive disorder,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B, vol. 264, no. 1389, pp. 1767–1773, 1997.
[27]
M. Abbruzzese, S. Ferri, and S. Scarone, “The selective breakdown of frontal functions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and in patients with schizophrenia: a double dissociation experimental finding,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 35, no. 6, pp. 907–912, 1997.
[28]
R. L. Albin and J. W. Mink, “Recent advances in Tourette syndrome research,” Trends in Neurosciences, vol. 29, no. 3, pp. 175–182, 2006.
[29]
H. S. Singer and J. T. Walkup, “Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders. Diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment,” Medicine, vol. 70, no. 1, pp. 15–32, 1991.
[30]
H. S. Singer, I. H. Hahn, and T. H. Moran, “Abnormal dopamine uptake sites in postmortem striatum from patients with Tourette's syndrome,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 558–562, 1991.
[31]
J. M. Delfs and A. E. Kelley, “The role of D1 and D2 dopamine receptors in oral stereotypy induced by dopaminergic stimulation of the ventrolateral striatum,” Neuroscience, vol. 39, no. 1, pp. 59–67, 1990.
[32]
M. Ernst, A. J. Zametkin, P. H. Jons, J. A. Matochik, D. Pascualvaca, and R. M. Cohen, “High presynaptic dopaminergic activity in children with Tourette's disorder,” Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 36–94, 1999.
[33]
N. Vermeulen, J. Godefroid, and M. Mermillod, “Emotional modulation of attention: fear increases but disgust reduces the attentional blink,” PLoS ONE, vol. 4, no. 11, Article ID e7924, 2009.
[34]
N. Vermeulen and M. Mermillod, “Fast emotional embodiment can modulate sensory exposure in perceivers,” Communicative and Integrative Biology, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 1–4, 2010.
[35]
N. Vermeulen, M. Mermillod, J. Godefroid, and O. Corneille, “Unintended embodiment of concepts into percepts: sensory activation boosts attention for same-modality concepts in the attentional blink paradigm,” Cognition, vol. 112, no. 3, pp. 467–472, 2009.
[36]
R. Sprengelmeyer, A. W. Young, K. Mahn et al., “Facial expression recognition in people with medicated and unmedicated Parkinson's disease,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 41, no. 8, pp. 1047–1057, 2003.
[37]
A. Suzuki, T. Hoshino, K. Shigemasu, and M. Kawamura, “Disgust-specific impairment of facial expression recognition in Parkinson's disease,” Brain, vol. 129, no. 3, pp. 707–717, 2006.
[38]
R. Adolphs, F. Gosselin, T. W. Buchanan, D. Tranel, P. Schyns, and A. R. Damasio, “A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage,” Nature, vol. 433, no. 7021, pp. 68–72, 2005.
[39]
N. A. Shapira, Y. Liu, A. G. He et al., “Brain activation by disgust-inducing pictures in obsessive-compulsive disorder,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 54, no. 7, pp. 751–756, 2003.
[40]
H. A. Parker, R. J. McNally, K. Nakayama, and S. Wilhelm, “No disgust recognition deficit in obsessive-compulsive disorder,” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, vol. 35, no. 2, pp. 183–192, 2004.
[41]
P. Ekman and W. V. Friesen, Pictures of Facial Affect, Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, Calif, USA, 1976.