全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Developing Attention: Behavioral and Brain Mechanisms

DOI: 10.1155/2014/405094

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Brain networks underlying attention are present even during infancy and are critical for the developing ability of children to control their emotions and thoughts. For adults, individual differences in the efficiency of attentional networks have been related to neuromodulators and to genetic variations. We have examined the development of attentional networks and child temperament in a longitudinal study from infancy (7 months) to middle childhood (7 years). Early temperamental differences among infants, including smiling and laughter and vocal reactivity, are related to self-regulation abilities at 7 years. However, genetic variations related to adult executive attention, while present in childhood, are poor predictors of later control, in part because individual genetic variation may have many small effects and in part because their influence occurs in interaction with caregiver behavior and other environmental influences. While brain areas involved in attention are present during infancy, their connectivity changes and leads to improvement in control of behavior. It is also possible to influence control mechanisms through training later in life. The relation between maturation and learning may allow advances in our understanding of human brain development. 1. Introduction Few life changes are as dramatic as the development that occurs between infancy and elementary school, with locomotion, language, and voluntary control as the most obvious behavior changes. We also know that the brain changes in size, connectivity, and synaptic density during this period. What is least explored is exactly how these brain changes support behavioral change. Our research traces the development of attention networks that support the mechanisms of self-regulation, allowing children to control their emotions and behavior. In this paper, we first outline the connection between attention and self-regulation. In the next section, we examine measurement of individual differences in attention in adults. The heart of the paper summarizes the relation of early temperament (7 months) to later temperament and attention (age: 7 years). We show how changes in mechanisms of control over this period relate to genes and to the environment provided by the caregiver. Finally, we examine training studies that influence some of the same brain connections that change during development. During infancy, the caregiver provides much of the child’s regulation. Soothing by holding and rocking or by orienting of attention is a common practice for control of distress. Holding supports the child’s

References

[1]  M. K. Rothbart, Becoming Who We Are: Temperament, Personality and Development, Guilford Press, New York, NY, USA, 2011.
[2]  M. I. Posner and S. E. Petersen, “The attention system of the human brain,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 13, pp. 25–42, 1990.
[3]  S. E. Petersen and M. I. Posner, “The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 35, pp. 73–89, 2012.
[4]  M. I. Posner, M. K. Rothbart, B. E. Sheese, and P. Voelker, “Control networks and neuromodulators of early development,” Developmental Psychology, vol. 48, no. 3, pp. 827–835, 2012.
[5]  M. K. Rothbart, B. E. Sheese, M. R. Rueda, and M. I. Posner, “Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life,” Emotion Review, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 207–213, 2011.
[6]  G. Bush, P. Luu, and M. I. Posner, “Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 4, no. 6, pp. 215–222, 2000.
[7]  S. A. Sheth, M. K. Mian, S. R. Patel et al., “Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioural adapation,” Nature, vol. 488, no. 7410, pp. 218–221, 2012.
[8]  P. Checa and M. R. Rueda, “Behavioral and brain measures of executive attention and school competence in late childhood,” Developmental Neuropsychology, vol. 36, no. 8, pp. 1018–1032, 2011.
[9]  T. E. Moffitt, L. Arseneault, D. Belsky et al., “A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 108, no. 7, pp. 2693–2698, 2011.
[10]  J. Fan, B. D. McCandliss, T. Sommer, A. Raz, and M. I. Posner, “Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 340–347, 2002.
[11]  M. R. Rueda, J. Fan, B. D. McCandliss et al., “Development of attentional networks in childhood,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 42, no. 8, pp. 1029–1040, 2004.
[12]  J. W. MacLeod, M. A. Lawrence, M. M. McConnell, G. A. Eskes, R. M. Klein, and D. I. Shore, “Appraising the ANT: psychometric and theoretical considerations of the Attention Network Test,” Neuropsychology, vol. 24, no. 5, pp. 637–651, 2010.
[13]  J. Fan, X. Gu, K. G. Guise et al., “Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 70, no. 2, pp. 209–220, 2009.
[14]  J. Fan, B. D. McCandliss, J. Fossella, J. I. Flombaum, and M. I. Posner, “The activation of attentional networks,” NeuroImage, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 471–479, 2005.
[15]  S. Niogi, P. Mukherjee, J. Ghajar, and B. D. McCandliss, “Individual differences in distinct components of attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinct white matter tracts,” Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, vol. 4, article 2, 2010.
[16]  L. T. Westlye, H. Grydeland, K. B. Walhovd, and A. M. Fjell, “Associations between regional cortical thickness and attentional networks as measured by the attention network test,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 21, no. 2, pp. 345–356, 2011.
[17]  A. Callejas, J. Lupiá?ez, and P. Tudela, “The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 54, no. 3, pp. 225–227, 2004.
[18]  M. Beckmann, H. Johansen-Berg, and M. F. S. Rushworth, “Connectivity-based parcellation of human cingulate cortex and its relation to functional specialization,” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 1175–1190, 2009.
[19]  M. I. Posner, B. E. Sheese, Y. Odluda?, and Y. Tang, “Analyzing and shaping human attentional networks,” Neural Networks, vol. 19, no. 9, pp. 1422–1429, 2006.
[20]  K. Supekar and V. Menon, “Developmental maturation of a dynamic causal control signals in higher-order cognition: a neurocognitive network model,” PLOS Computational Biology, vol. 8, no. 2, Article ID e1002374, 2012.
[21]  T. E. Behrens, “Neural mechanisms underlying human choice in frontal cortex,” in Neurosciences and the Human Person: New on Human Activities, W. Singer S, Dehaene, and A. Batro, Eds., Pontifical Academy of Sciences, Vatican, 2014.
[22]  K. Zilles, “Evolution of the human brain and comparative syto and receptor architecture,” in From Monkey Brain to Human Brain, S. Dehaene, J. R. Duhamel, M. D. Rizzolatti, and G. Rizzolatti, Eds., pp. 41–56, MIT Press, Cambridge Mass, 2005.
[23]  J. M. Allman, K. K. Watson, N. A. Tetreault, and A. Y. Hakeem, “Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von Economo neurons,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 9, no. 8, pp. 367–373, 2005.
[24]  N. U. F. Dosenbach, D. A. Fair, F. M. Miezin et al., “Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 104, no. 26, pp. 11073–11078, 2007.
[25]  B. J. Casey, L. H. Somerville, I. H. Gotlib et al., “Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 108, no. 36, pp. 14998–15003, 2011.
[26]  B. E. Sheese, M. K. Rothbart, M. I. Posner, L. K. White, and S. H. Fraundorf, “Executive attention and self-regulation in infancy,” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 31, no. 3, pp. 501–510, 2008.
[27]  C. Harman, M. K. Rothbart, and M. I. Posner, “Distress and attention interactions in early infancy,” Motivation and Emotion, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 27–43, 1997.
[28]  A. E. Green, M. R. Munafò, C. G. DeYoung, J. A. Fossella, J. Fan, and J. R. Gray, “Using genetic data in cognitive neuroscience: from growing pains to genuine insights,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 9, no. 9, pp. 710–720, 2008.
[29]  D. J. Lewkowicz and A. M. Hansen-Tift, “Infants deploy selective attention to the mouth of a talking face when learning speech,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 109, no. 5, pp. 1431–1436, 2012.
[30]  W. Gao, H. Zhu, K. S. Giovanello et al., “Evidence on the emergence of the brain's default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 106, no. 16, pp. 6790–6795, 2009.
[31]  P. Fransson, U. ?den, M. Blennow, and H. Lagercrantz, “The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI,” Cerebral Cortex, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 145–154, 2011.
[32]  V. Menon, “Developmental pathways to function brain networks: emerging principles,” Trends in Cognitive Science, vol. 17, no. 12, pp. 627–639, 2013.
[33]  J. D. Power, K. A. Barnes, A. Z. Snyder, B. L. Schlaggar, and S. E. Petersen, “Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion,” NeuroImage, vol. 59, no. 3, pp. 2142–2154, 2012.
[34]  L. Q. Uddin, K. S. Supekar, S. Ryali, and V. Menon, “Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development,” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 31, no. 50, pp. 18578–18589, 2011.
[35]  A. Berger, G. Tzur, and M. I. Posner, “Infant brains detect arithmetic errors,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 103, no. 33, pp. 12649–12653, 2006.
[36]  K. Wynn, “Addition and subtraction by human infants,” Nature, vol. 358, no. 6389, pp. 749–750, 1992.
[37]  P. M. A. Rabbitt, “Three kinds of error-signalling responses in a serial choice task,” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 179–188, 1968.
[38]  L. B. Jones, M. K. Rothbart, and M. I. Posner, “Development of executive attention in preschool children,” Developmental Science, vol. 6, no. 5, pp. 498–504, 2003.
[39]  B. A. Eriksen and C. W. Eriksen, “Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task,” Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 143–149, 1974.
[40]  A. M. Fjell, K. B. Walhovd, T. T. Brown, et al., “Multi modal imaging of the self-regulating brain,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 109, no. 48, pp. 19620–19625, 2012.
[41]  S. B. Perlman and K. A. Pelphrey, “Developing connections for affective regulation: age-related changes in emotional brain connectivity,” Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol. 108, no. 3, pp. 607–620, 2011.
[42]  A. Etkin, T. Egner, D. M. Peraza, E. R. Kandel, and J. Hirsch, “Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala,” Neuron, vol. 51, no. 6, pp. 871–882, 2006.
[43]  S. Crottaz-Herbette and V. Menon, “Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulates attentional response: combined fMRI and ERP evidence,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 766–780, 2006.
[44]  S. Dehaene, M. I. Posner, and D. M. Tucker, “Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation,” Psychological Science, vol. 5, no. 5, pp. 303–305, 1994.
[45]  M. R. Rueda, M. K. Rothbart, B. D. McCandliss, L. Saccomanno, and M. I. Posner, “Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102, no. 41, pp. 14931–14936, 2005.
[46]  M. R. Rueda, P. Checa, and L. M. Cómbita, “Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: immediate changes and effects after two months,” Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 2, supplement 1, pp. S192–S204, 2012.
[47]  M. K. Rothbart and D. Derryberry, “Development of individual differences in temperament,” in Advances in Developmental Psychology, M. E. Lamb and A. L. Brown, Eds., pp. 37–86, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1981.
[48]  S. P. Putnam, M. K. Rothbart, and M. A. Gartstein, “Homotypic and heterotypic continuity of fine-grained temperament during infancy, toddlerhood, and early childhood,” Infant and Child Development, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 387–405, 2008.
[49]  N. Komsi, K. R?ikk?nen, A.-K. Pesonen et al., “Continuity of temperament from infancy to middle childhood,” Infant Behavior and Development, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 494–508, 2006.
[50]  M. Reuter, U. Ott, D. Vaitl, and J. Hennig, “Impaired executive control is associated with a variation in the promoter region of the tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 401–408, 2007.
[51]  S. A. Markett, C. Montag, and M. Reuter, “The association between dopamine DRD2 polymorphisms and working memory capacity is modulated by a functional polymorphism on the nicotinic receptor gene CHRNA4,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 22, no. 9, pp. 1944–1954, 2010.
[52]  B. E. Sheese, P. M. Voelker, M. K. Rothbart, and M. I. Posner, “Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood,” Development and Psychopathology, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 1039–1046, 2007.
[53]  P. Voelker, B. E. Sheese, M. K. Rothbart, and M. I. Posner, “Variations in catechol-O-methyltransferase gene interact with parenting to influence attention in early development,” Neuroscience, vol. 164, no. 1, pp. 121–130, 2009.
[54]  B. E. Sheese, M. K. Rothbart, P. Voelker, and M. I. Posner, “The dopamine receptor D4 gene 7 repeat allele interacts with parenting quality to predict Effortful Control in four-year- old children,” Child Development Research, vol. 2012, Article ID 863242, 6 pages, 2012.
[55]  H. J. Smith, H. I. Sheikh, M. W. Dyson et al., “Parenting and child DRD4 genotype interact to predict children’s early emerging Effortful Control,” Child Development, vol. 83, no. 6, pp. 1932–1944, 2012.
[56]  NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, “The NICHD Study of Early Child Care: a comprehensive longitudinal study of young children's lives,” ERIC Document Reproduction Service ED3530870, 1993.
[57]  B. E. Sheese, P. Voelker, M. I. Posner, and M. K. Rothbart, “Genetic variation influences on the early development of reactive emotions and their regulation by attention,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 14, no. 4-5, pp. 332–355, 2009.
[58]  E. M. Tucker-Drob and D. A. Briley, “Continuity of genetic and environmental influences on cognition across the life span: a meta analysis of longitudinal twin and adoption studies,” Psychological Bulletin, 2014.
[59]  D. A. Fair, A. L. Cohen, J. D. Power et al., “Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization,” PLoS Computational Biology, vol. 5, no. 5, Article ID e1000381, 2009.
[60]  C. Lebel, M. Gee, R. Camicioli, M. Wieler, W. Martin, and C. Beaulieu, “Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan,” NeuroImage, vol. 60, no. 1, pp. 340–352, 2012.
[61]  Y.-Y. Tang, Y. Ma, J. Wang et al., “Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 104, no. 43, pp. 17152–17156, 2007.
[62]  Y.-Y. Tang, Q. Lu, X. Geng, E. A. Stein, Y. Yang, and M. I. Posner, “Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in the anterior cingulate,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 107, no. 35, pp. 15649–15652, 2010.
[63]  I. J. Bennett, D. J. Madden, C. J. Vaidya, D. V. Howard, and J. H. Howard Jr., “Age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity: a diffusion tensor imaging study of healthy aging,” Human Brain Mapping, vol. 31, no. 3, pp. 378–390, 2010.
[64]  A. Z. Burzynska, C. Preuschhof, L. B?ckman et al., “Age-related differences in white matter microstructure: region-specific patterns of diffusivity,” NeuroImage, vol. 49, no. 3, pp. 2104–2112, 2010.
[65]  Y.-Y. Tang, Q. Lu, M. Fan, Y. Yang, and M. I. Posner, “Mechanisms of white matter changes induced by meditation,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 109, no. 26, pp. 10570–10574, 2012.
[66]  N. Provencal, M. J. Suderman, C. Guillemin, et al., “The signature of maternal rearing in the methylome in rhesus macaque prefrontal cortex and T cells,” The Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 32, no. 44, pp. 15626–15642, 2012.
[67]  N. Proven?al, M. J. Suderman, F. Vitaro, M. Szyf, and R. E. Tremblay, “Childhood chronic physical aggression associates with adult cytokine levels in plasma,” PLos ONE, vol. 8, no. 7, Article ID e69481, 2013.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133