When playing violent video games, aggressive actions are performed against the background of an originally neutral environment, and associations are formed between cues related to violence and contextual features. This experiment examined the hypothesis that neutral contextual features of a virtual environment become associated with aggressive meaning and acquire the function of primes for aggressive cognitions. Seventy-six participants were assigned to one of two violent video game conditions that varied in context (ship vs. city environment) or a control condition. Afterwards, they completed a Lexical Decision Task to measure the accessibility of aggressive cognitions in which they were primed either with ship-related or city-related words. As predicted, participants who had played the violent game in the ship environment had shorter reaction times for aggressive words following the ship primes than the city primes, whereas participants in the city condition responded faster to the aggressive words following the city primes compared to the ship primes. No parallel effect was observed for the non-aggressive targets. The findings indicate that the associations between violent and neutral cognitions learned during violent game play facilitate the accessibility of aggressive cognitions.
References
[1]
Connolly, T.M.; Boyle, E.A.; MacArthur, E.; Hainey, T.; Boyle, J.M. A systematic literature review of empirical evidence on computer games and serious games. Comput. Educ. 2012, 59, 661–686, doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.03.004.
[2]
Gross, M.L. Advergames and the effects of game-product congruity. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2010, 26, 1259–1265, doi:10.1016/j.chb.2010.03.034.
[3]
Smith, S.L.; Lachlan, K.; Tamborini, R. Popular video games: Quantifying the presentation of violence and its context. J. Broadcast. Electron. 2003, 47, 58–76, doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem4701_4.
Todorov, A.; Bargh, J.A. Automatic sources of aggression. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2002, 7, 53–68, doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(00)00036-7.
[6]
Berkowitz, L.; LePage, A. Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1967, 7, 202–207, doi:10.1037/h0025008.
[7]
Anderson, C.A.; Benjamin, A.J., Jr.; Bartholow, B.D. Does the gun pull the trigger? Automatic priming effects of weapon pictures and weapon names. Psychol. Sci. 1998, 9, 308–314, doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00061.
[8]
Bartholow, B.D.; Anderson, C.A.; Carnagey, N.L.; Arlin, B.J.J. Interactive effects of life experience and situational cues on aggression: The weapons priming effect in hunters and nonhunters. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 41, 48–60, doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.05.005.
[9]
De Wall, C.N.; Bushman, B.J. Hot under the collar in a lukewarm environment: Words associated with hot temperature increase aggressive thoughts and hostile perceptions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 1045–1047, doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.003.
[10]
Devine, P.G. Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 5–18, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5.
[11]
Bartholow, B.D.; Heinz, A. Alcohol and aggression without consumption: Alcohol cues, aggressive thoughts, and hostile perception bias. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 30–37, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01661.x.
[12]
Bargh, J.A.; Chen, M.; Burrows, L. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71, 230–244, doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230.
[13]
Van Overwalle, F. Introduction and overview. In Social Connectionism: A Reader and Handbook for Simulations; Van Overwalle, F., Ed.; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
Anderson, C.A.; Shibuya, A.; Ihori, N.; Swing, E.L.; Bushman, B.J.; Sakamoto, A.; Rothstein, H.R.; Saleem, M. Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 151–173.
[16]
Berkowitz, L. Aggression: Its Causes, Consequences, and Control; Temple University Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1993.
[17]
B?sche, W. Violent video games prime both aggressive and positive cognitions. J Media Psychol. 2010, 22, 139–146, doi:10.1027/1864-1105/a000019.
[18]
Carnagey, N.L.; Anderson, C.A. The effects of reward and punishment in violent video games on aggressive affect, cognition, and behavior. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 882–889, doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01632.x.
[19]
Anderson, C.A.; Carnagey, N.L. Causal effects of violent sports video games on aggression: Is it competitiveness or violent content? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 45, 731–739, doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.019.
[20]
Busching, R.; Gentile, D.; Krahé, B.; M?ller, I.; Khoo, A.; Walsh, D.A.; Anderson, C.A. Testing the reliability and validity of different measures of violent video game use in the USA, Singapore, and Germany. Psych. Pop. Media Cult. 2013, doi:10.1037/ppm0000004.
[21]
Neely, J. Semantic Priming Effects in Visual Word Recognition: A Selective Review of Current Findings and Theories. In Basic Processes in Reading: Visual Word Recognition; Besner, D., Humphreys, G.W., Eds.; L. Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991.
[22]
Krahé, B.; M?ller, I.; Huesmann, L.R.; Kirwil, L.; Felber, J.; Berger, A. Desensitization to media violence: Links with habitual media violence exposure, aggressive cognitions, and aggressive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 630–646, doi:10.1037/a0021711.
[23]
Valve Corporation, Counter-Strike: Source. [Video Game], Electronic Arts (EA), Bellevue, WA, USA, 2011.
[24]
Kinoshita, S.; Mozer, M.C.; Forster, K.I. Dynamic adaptation to history of trial difficulty explains the effect of congruency proportion on masked priming. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2011, 140, 622–636, doi:10.1037/a0024230.
[25]
Kliegl, R.; Masson, M.E.J.; Richter, E.M. A linear mixed model analysis of masked repetition priming. Vis. Cogn. 2010, 18, 655–681, doi:10.1080/13506280902986058.
[26]
Gelman, A.B.; Hill, J. Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models, 1st ed. ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, NY, USA, 2007.
[27]
Huesmann, L.R. An information processing model for the development of aggression. Aggr. Behav. 1988, 14, 13–24, doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1988)14:1<13::AID-AB2480140104>3.0.CO;2-J.
[28]
Krahé, B.; M?ller, I. Playing violent electronic games, hostile attribution bias and aggression-related norms in German adolescents. J. Adolescence 2004, 27, 53–69, doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.006.
[29]
Kirsh, S.J. Seeing the world through mortal kombat-colored glasses: Violent video games and the development of a short-term hostile attribution bias. Childhood 1998, 5, 177–184, doi:10.1177/0907568298005002005.
[30]
Berkowitz, L.; Geen, R.G. Film violence and the cue properties of available targets. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1966, 3, 525–530, doi:10.1037/h0023201.