The Social Attribution Task-Multiple Choice (SAT-MC) uses a 64-second video of geometric shapes set in motion to portray themes of social relatedness and intentions. Considered a test of “Theory of Mind,” the SAT-MC assesses implicit social attribution formation while reducing verbal and basic cognitive demands required of other common measures. We present a comparability analysis of the SAT-MC and the new SAT-MC-II, an alternate form created for repeat testing, in a university sample ( ). Score distributions and patterns of association with external validation measures were nearly identical between the two forms, with convergent and discriminant validity supported by association with affect recognition ability and lack of association with basic visual reasoning. Internal consistency of the SAT-MC-II was superior (alpha = .81) to the SAT-MC (alpha = .56). Results support the use of SAT-MC and new SAT-MC-II as equivalent test forms. Demonstrating relatively higher association to social cognitive than basic cognitive abilities, the SAT-MC may provide enhanced sensitivity as an outcome measure of social cognitive intervention trials. 1. Introduction Human social behavior involves a complex interchange between self-perception, knowledge of social expectations, interpretation of subtle social cues, and inferences about the mental state of others. The study of social information processing, broadly termed social cognition [1], has increasingly become a priority area in psychiatric research and assessment. The profound effects of social impairment in schizophrenia may in part be explained by difficulties interpreting the emotions and actions of others and regulating one’s own behavior in response to a dynamic social world. There is now considerable evidence to suggest that laboratory-based assessment of social cognition improves prediction of real world functional outcomes beyond what is captured by standard neuropsychological assessment [2–5]. Growing appreciation for the complexity of social behavior has guided the development of assessment methods to target discrete domains of social information processing, of which emotion processing, social perception, social knowledge, theory of mind (ToM), and attributional bias are the most widely studied in schizophrenia [1]. Despite general consensus regarding this categorization of social cognitive domains, determination of functional associations distinctly related to social information processing may be obscured by a generalized neurocognitive deficit common to this population [6–8]. Generalized deficit may
References
[1]
M. F. Green, D. L. Penn, R. Bentall et al., “Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 34, no. 6, pp. 1211–1220, 2008.
[2]
J. S. Brekke, M. Hoe, J. Long, and M. F. Green, “How neurocognition and social cognition influence functional change during community-based psychosocial rehabilitation for individuals with schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 33, no. 5, pp. 1247–1256, 2007.
[3]
S. M. Couture, D. L. Penn, and D. L. Roberts, “The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, supplement 1, pp. S44–S63, 2006.
[4]
J. M. Fiszdon and J. K. Johannesen, “Functional significance of preserved affect recognition in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 176, no. 2-3, pp. 120–125, 2010.
[5]
C. Hooker and S. Park, “Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia patients,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 112, no. 1, pp. 41–50, 2002.
[6]
S. L. Kerr and J. M. Neale, “Emotion perception in schizophrenia: specific deficit or further evidence of generalized poor performance?” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 102, no. 2, pp. 312–318, 1993.
[7]
D. L. Penn, D. R. Combs, M. Ritchie et al., “Emotion recognition in schizophrenia: further investigation of generalized versus specific deficit models,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 109, no. 3, pp. 512–516, 2000.
[8]
S. M. Silverstein, “Information processing, social cognition, and psychiatric rehabilitation in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry, vol. 60, no. 4, pp. 327–340, 1997.
[9]
G. Bryson, M. Bell, and P. Lysaker, “Affect recognition in schizophrenia: a function of global impairment or a specific cognitive deficit,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 71, no. 2, pp. 105–113, 1997.
[10]
V. P. Bozikas, M. H. Kosmidis, D. Anezoulaki, M. Giannakou, and A. Karavatos, “Relationship of affect recognition with psychopathology and cognitive performance in schizophrenia,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 549–558, 2004.
[11]
L. Harrington, R. J. Siegert, and J. McClure, “Theory of mind in schizophrenia: a critical review,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 10, no. 4, pp. 249–286, 2005.
[12]
E. Bora, M. Yucel, and C. Pantelis, “Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 109, no. 1–3, pp. 1–9, 2009.
[13]
T. C. Greig, G. J. Bryson, and M. D. Bell, “Theory of mind performance in schizophrenia: diagnostic, symptom, and neuropsychological correlates,” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, vol. 192, no. 1, pp. 12–18, 2004.
[14]
J. Addington, H. Saeedi, and D. Addington, “Facial affect recognition: a mediator between cognitive and social functioning in psychosis?” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 85, no. 1–3, pp. 142–150, 2006.
[15]
M. Bell, H. W. H. Tsang, T. C. Greig, and G. J. Bryson, “Neurocognition, social cognition, perceived social discomfort, and vocational outcomes in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 35, no. 4, pp. 738–747, 2009.
[16]
S. J. Schmidt, D. R. Mueller, and V. Roder, “Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 37, supplement 2, no. 2, pp. S41–S54, 2011.
[17]
M. J. Sergi, Y. Rassovsky, K. H. Nuechterlein, and M. F. Green, “Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 163, no. 3, pp. 448–454, 2006.
[18]
W. P. Horan, R. S. Kern, M. F. Green, and D. L. Penn, “Social cognition training for individuals with schizophrenia: emerging evidence,” American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 205–252, 2008.
[19]
F. Heider and M. Simmel, “An experimental study of apparent behavior,” American Journal of Psychology, vol. 57, pp. 243–259, 1944.
[20]
M. Streit, W. W?lwer, and W. Gaebel, “Facial-affect recognitionand visual scanning behaviour in the course of schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 24, no. 3, pp. 311–317, 1997.
[21]
B. I. Turetsky, C. G. Kohler, T. Indersmitten, M. T. Bhati, D. Charbonnier, and R. C. Gur, “Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia:when and why does it go awry?” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 94, no. 1–3, pp. 253–263, 2007.
[22]
S. Walther, A. Federspiel, H. Horn et al., “Encoding deficit during face processing within the right fusiform face area in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 172, no. 3, pp. 184–191, 2009.
[23]
A. Klin, “Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning Autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task,” Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, vol. 47, no. 7, pp. 831–846, 2000.
[24]
A. Klin and W. Jones, “Attributing social and physical meaning to ambiguous visual displays in individuals with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders,” Brain and Cognition, vol. 61, no. 1, pp. 40–53, 2006.
[25]
M. D. Bell, J. M. Fiszdon, T. C. Greig, and B. E. Wexler, “Social attribution test—multiple choice (SAT-MC) in schizophrenia: comparison with community sample and relationship to neurocognitive, social cognitive and symptom measures,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 122, no. 1–3, pp. 164–171, 2010.
[26]
M. L. Eckblad, L. J. Chapman, J. P. Chapman, and M. Mishlove, The Revised Social Anhedonia Scale, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wis, USA, 1982.
[27]
A. Raine, “The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 555–564, 1991.
[28]
E. Fonseca-Pedrero, M. Paíno-Pi?eiro, S. Lemos-Giráldez, ú. Villazón-García, and J. Mu?iz, “Validation of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire-Brief form in adolescents,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 111, no. 1–3, pp. 53–60, 2009.
[29]
M. D. Bell, Bell Relationship Inventory for Adolescents (BRIA) Manual, Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, Calif, USA, 2005.
[30]
M. Bell, G. Bryson, and P. Lysaker, “Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 73, no. 1-2, pp. 73–82, 1997.
[31]
D. Wechsler, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Psychological Corperation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 3rd edition, 1997.
[32]
J. H. Steiger, “Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 87, no. 2, pp. 245–251, 1980.
[33]
D. A. Hackman and M. J. Farah, “Socioeconomic status and the developing brain,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 65–73, 2009.
[34]
K. G. Noble, M. F. Norman, and M. J. Farah, “Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children,” Developmental Science, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 74–87, 2005.