The majority of functional neuroimaging studies investigating neural correlates of emotion processing in schizophrenia report a significant deficit in limbic structures activation in patients relative to control participants. Recently it has been suggested that this apparent “deficit” could be due to an enhanced sensitivity of the neutral material in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia, rather than due to their inefficiency in emotion processing. The purpose of the present study was to test this supposition and verify if the potential effect is present in both men and women diagnosed with schizophrenia. In order to do that we examined the pattern of cerebral activation associated with processing of neutral stimuli in schizophrenia. Thirty-seven schizophrenia patients and 37 healthy controls viewed neutral and emotional images while in a functional magnetic resonance imaging scanner. Schizophrenia patients rated the neutral images as more emotionally salient than controls. Additionally, patients showed significant activation during processing of neutral images in limbic and prefrontal regions; similar areas were underactivated in patients relative to controls during processing of emotional information. Investigation of sex differences revealed that the enhanced responsiveness to the emotionally neutral material was attributed primarily to men with schizophrenia. 1. Introduction Functional neuroimaging studies, which explored processing of emotional material in schizophrenia, have often reported “deficits” in cerebral activation in patients compared to control participants in various limbic, paralimbic, and prefrontal regions (e.g., [1–4]). A few other studies documented “abnormal overactivation” in patients relative to controls or no difference between the groups [5–10]. These divergent findings have been attributed to the type of emotional task (passive viewing, emotion identification, emotional memory, etc.) and to the characteristics of the recruited patients (first-episode versus chronic, medicated versus unmediated, presence of prominent negative versus prominent positive symptoms, etc.). However, what could have played an equally or even more important role in the obtained results is the kind of functional neuroimaging contrast used in the statistical analysis. It should be pointed out to readers less familiar with the functional neuroimaging literature that the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies in various clinical populations (including schizophrenia and related psychoses) have relied primarily on comparisons between two
References
[1]
R. E. Gur, C. McGrath, R. M. Chan et al., “An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 159, no. 12, pp. 1992–1999, 2002.
[2]
S. Paradiso, N. C. Andreasen, B. Crespo-Facorro et al., “Emotions in unmedicated patients with schizophrenia during evaluation with positron emission tomography,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 160, no. 10, pp. 1775–1783, 2003.
[3]
F. Schneider, U. Weiss, C. Kessler et al., “Differential amygdala activation in schizophrenia during sadness,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 133–142, 1998.
[4]
H. Takahashi, M. Koeda, K. Oda et al., “An fMRI study of differential neural response to affective pictures in schizophrenia,” NeuroImage, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 1247–1254, 2004.
[5]
E. C. Dowd and D. M. Barch, “Anhedonia and emotional experience in schizophrenia: neural and behavioral indicators,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 67, no. 10, pp. 902–911, 2010.
[6]
A. Hempel, E. Hempel, P. Sch?nknecht, C. Stippich, and J. Schr?der, “Impairment in basal limbic function in schizophrenia during affect recognition,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 122, no. 2, pp. 115–124, 2003.
[7]
H. Kosaka, M. Omori, T. Murata et al., “Differential amygdala response during facial recognition in patients with schizophrenia: an fMRI study,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 57, no. 1, pp. 87–95, 2002.
[8]
M. Reske, U. Habel, T. Kellermann et al., “Differential brain activation during facial emotion discrimination in first-episode schizophrenia,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 43, no. 6, pp. 592–599, 2009.
[9]
K. Sergerie, J. L. Armony, M. Menear, H. Sutton, and M. Lepage, “Influence of emotional expression on memory recognition bias in schizophrenia as revealed by fMRI,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 800–810, 2010.
[10]
S. F. Taylor, I. Liberzon, L. R. Decker, and R. A. Koeppe, “A functional anatomic study of emotion in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 58, no. 2-3, pp. 159–172, 2002.
[11]
A. Anticevic, J. X. Van Snellenberg, R. E. Cohen, G. Repovs, E. C. Dowd, and D. M. Barch, “Amygdala recruitment in schizophrenia in response to aversive emotional material: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 38, no. 3, pp. 608–621, 2012.
[12]
J. Hall, H. C. Whalley, J. W. McKirdy et al., “Overactivation of fear systems to neutral faces in schizophrenia,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 70–73, 2008.
[13]
D. J. Holt, L. Kunkel, A. P. Weiss et al., “Increased medial temporal lobe activation during the passive viewing of emotional and neutral facial expressions in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 82, no. 2-3, pp. 153–162, 2006.
[14]
N. Lakis, J. A. Jiménez, A. Mancini-Mar?e, E. Stip, M. E. Lavoie, and A. Mendrek, “Neural correlates of emotional recognition memory in schizophrenia: effects of valence and arousal,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 194, no. 3, pp. 245–256, 2011.
[15]
S. Surguladze, T. Russell, K. Kucharska-Pietura et al., “A reversal of the normal pattern of parahippocampal response to neutral and fearful faces is associated with reality distortion in schizophrenia,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 60, no. 5, pp. 423–431, 2006.
[16]
APA, American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Press, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, 1994.
[17]
R. C. Oldfield, “The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 97–113, 1971.
[18]
Census, The National Occupational Classification and the Career Handbook, 2001, http://www5.hrsdc.gc.ca/noc/English/CH/2001/IntroductionNOCandCH.aspx.
[19]
R. L. Spitzer, J. B. W. Williams, M. Gibbon, and M. B. First, “The structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): I: history, rationale, and description,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 49, no. 8, pp. 624–629, 1992.
[20]
S. R. Kay, A. Fiszbein, and L. A. Opler, “The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 261–276, 1987.
[21]
S. W. Woods, “Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 64, no. 6, pp. 663–667, 2003.
[22]
K. J. Friston, “Commentary and opinion: II. Statistical parametric mapping: ontology and current issues,” Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 361–370, 1995.
[23]
B. Walter, C. Blecker, P. Kirsch et al., “MARINA: an easy to use tool for the creation of masks for region of interest analyses,” in Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, vol. 19, New York, NY, USA, June 2003, available on CD-Rom in NeuroImage.
[24]
N. Tzourio-Mazoyer, B. Landeau, D. Papathanassiou et al., “Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain,” NeuroImage, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 273–289, 2002.
[25]
J.-B. Poline, K. J. Worsley, A. C. Evans, and K. J. Friston, “Combining spatial extent and peak intensity to test for activations in functional imaging,” NeuroImage, vol. 5, no. 2, pp. 83–96, 1997.
[26]
M. L. Phillips, L. Williams, C. Senior et al., “A differential neural response to threatening and non-threatening negative facial expressions in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 92, no. 1, pp. 11–31, 1999.
[27]
F. Schneider, W. Grodd, U. Weiss et al., “Functional MRI reveals left amygdala activation during emotion,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 76, no. 2-3, pp. 75–82, 1997.
[28]
F. Schneider, U. Habel, M. Reske, I. Toni, P. Falkai, and N. J. Shah, “Neural substrates of olfactory processing in schizophrenia patients and their healthy relatives,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 155, no. 2, pp. 103–112, 2007.
[29]
P. G. Michalopoulou, S. Surguladze, L. A. Morley, V. P. Giampietro, R. M. Murray, and S. S. Shergill, “Facial fear processing and psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia: functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 192, no. 3, pp. 191–196, 2008.
[30]
K. L. Phan, T. Wager, S. F. Taylor, and I. Liberzon, “Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI,” NeuroImage, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 331–348, 2002.
[31]
P. C. Fletcher, T. Shallice, and R. J. Dolan, “The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. I. Encoding,” Brain, vol. 121, no. 7, pp. 1239–1248, 1998.
[32]
M. R. Uncapher and A. D. Wagner, “Posterior parietal cortex and episodic encoding: insights from fMRI subsequent memory effects and dual-attention theory,” Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, vol. 91, no. 2, pp. 139–154, 2009.
[33]
A. Mendrek, N. Lakis, and J. Jiménez, “Associations of sex steroid hormones with cerebral activations during mental rotation in men and women with schizophrenia,” Psychoneuroendocrinology, vol. 36, no. 9, pp. 1422–1426, 2011.
[34]
A. Mendrek and E. Stip, “Sexual dimorphism in schizophrenia: is there a need for gender-based protocols?” Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, vol. 11, no. 7, pp. 951–959, 2011.
[35]
O. D. Howes and S. Kapur, “The dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: version III—the final common pathway,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 549–562, 2009.
[36]
S. Kapur, “Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 160, no. 1, pp. 13–23, 2003.
[37]
N. Y. Seiferth, K. Pauly, U. Habel et al., “Increased neural response related to neutral faces in individuals at risk for psychosis,” NeuroImage, vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 289–297, 2008.
[38]
S. F. Taylor, K. L. Phan, J. C. Britton, and I. Liberzon, “Neural response to emotional salience in schizophrenia,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 30, no. 5, pp. 984–995, 2005.
[39]
M. L. Phillips, C. Senior, and A. S. David, “Perception of threat in schizophrenics with persecutory delusions: an investigation using visual scan paths,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 157–167, 2000.