Introduction. The nocebo effect consists in delivering verbal suggestions of negative outcomes so that the subject expects clinical worsening. Several studies indicate that negative verbal suggestions may result in the amplification of pain. Amplification style is one of the most important dimensions in psychosomatic research. Methods. One group of pain therapy unit patients was evaluated at baseline and again after 6 months from the beginning of the pain treatment. Results. Only 43% of 86 chronic pain patients respond positively to the expectation of sham pain. This group shows at baseline higher pain intensity (t value: 2.72, ) and lower cold pain threshold (t value: 2.18, ) than the group of subjects with any response to sham pain stimulus. Somatoform dimensions influence positively the strength of nocebo response in those predisposed to it. Conclusion. Our study shows that the power of the nocebo phenomenon seems to be a dimension belonging to the investigation in psychosomatic. In contrast to what one might expect, the presence of the nocebo phenomenon affects positively pain relief and the outcome of pain treatment. In a clinical setting, and the meaning of nocebo response does not seem to be different from placebo response. 1. Introduction The nocebo effect consists in delivering verbal suggestions of negative outcomes so that the subject expects clinical worsening [1]. Several studies indicate that negative verbal suggestions may result in the amplification of pain [2, 3] and in the alteration of somatosensory perception [4]. In order to try to disentangle the effects of positive and negative cues on pain processing, several authors used brain imaging [5–7]. Expectation of pain increase has been found to enhance the activation of the thalamus, insula, prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex [6], but it is not known how this variation may influence pain processing in clinical pain [8]. Previous studies suggest that nocebo effects, sometimes termed “negative placebo effects,” contribute appreciably to a variety of medical symptoms [9, 10] adverse events in clinical trials and medical care [11–14], and public health “mass psychogenic illness” outbreaks [15]. While the majority of studies on placebo and nocebo effects show consistent results with patient expectations and the main psychological mechanisms, they seem to be the related factors of subconscious conditioning and conscious expectations [8].Kennedy introduced the concept of “nocebo reaction” in 1961, a few years after Beecher published his landmark paper on the placebo effect.
References
[1]
L. Colloca, M. Sigaudo, and F. Benedetti, “The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects,” Pain, vol. 136, no. 1-2, pp. 211–218, 2008.
[2]
T. Koyama, Y. Z. Tanaka, and A. Mikami, “Nociceptive neurons in the macaque anterior cingulate activate during anticipation of pain,” NeuroReport, vol. 9, no. 11, pp. 2663–2667, 1998.
[3]
D. D. Price, “Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain,” Science, vol. 288, no. 5472, pp. 1769–1772, 2000.
[4]
N. Sawamoto, M. Honda, T. Okada et al., “Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 20, no. 19, pp. 7438–7445, 2000.
[5]
J. R. Keltner, A. Furst, C. Fan, R. Redfern, B. Inglis, and H. L. Fields, “Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 26, no. 16, pp. 4437–4443, 2006.
[6]
T. Koyama, J. G. McHaffie, P. J. Laurienti, and R. C. Coghill, “The subjective experience of pain: where expectations became reality,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 102, pp. 12950–12955, 2005.
[7]
J. Lorenz, M. Hauck, R. C. Paur et al., “Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments—a possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions,” Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 283–295, 2005.
[8]
L. Manchikanti, J. Giordano, B. Fellows, and J. A. Hirsch, “Placebo and nocebo in interventional pain management: a friend or a foe—or simply foes?” Pain Physician, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. E157–E175, 2011.
[9]
A. J. Barsky and J. F. Borus, “Functional somatic syndromes,” Annals of Internal Medicine, vol. 130, no. 11, pp. 910–921, 1999.
[10]
A. J. Barsky, R. Saintfort, M. P. Rogers, and J. F. Borus, “Nonspecific medication side effects and the nocebo phenomenon,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 287, no. 5, pp. 622–627, 2002.
[11]
M. G. Myers, J. A. Cairns, and J. Singer, “The consent form as a possible cause of side effects,” Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 250–253, 1987.
[12]
J. A. Roscoe, J. T. Hickok, and G. R. Morrow, “Patient expectations as predictor of chemotherapy-induced nausea,” Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 22, no. 2, pp. 121–126, 2000.
[13]
U. Reuter, M. S. Del Rio, J. A. Carpay, C. J. Boes, and S. D. Silberstein, “Placebo adverse events in headache trials: headache as an adverse event of placebo,” Cephalalgia, vol. 23, no. 7, pp. 496–503, 2003.
[14]
T. J. Kaptchuk, W. B. Stason, R. B. Davis et al., “Sham device v inert pill: randomised controlled trial of two placebo treatments,” British Medical Journal, vol. 332, no. 7538, pp. 391–397, 2006.
[15]
T. F. Jones, A. S. Craig, D. Hoy et al., “Mass psychogenic illness attributed to toxic exposure at a high school,” New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 342, no. 2, pp. 96–100, 2000.
[16]
W. P. Kennedy, “The nocebo reaction,” Medical World, vol. 95, pp. 203–205, 1961.
[17]
H. K. Beecher, “The powerful placebo,” Journal of the American Medical Association, vol. 159, no. 17, pp. 1602–1606, 1955.
[18]
A. Stoudemire, Psychological Factors Affecting Medical Conditions, American Psychiatric Press, Arlington, VA, USA, 1995.
[19]
G. A. Fava, S. Fabbri, L. Sirri, and T. N. Wise, “Psychological factors affecting medical condition: a new proposal for DSM-V,” Psychosomatics, vol. 48, no. 2, pp. 103–111, 2007.
[20]
T. L. Bayer, P. E. Baer, and C. Early, “Situational and psychophysiological factors in psychologically induced pain,” Pain, vol. 44, no. 1, pp. 45–50, 1991.
[21]
T. L. Bayer, J. H. Coverdale, E. Chiang, and M. Bangs, “The role of prior pain experience and expectancy in psychologically and physically induced pain,” Pain, vol. 74, no. 2-3, pp. 327–331, 1998.
[22]
B. B. Wolff, “Methods of testing pain mechanisms in normal man,” in Textbook of Pain, P. D. Wall and R. Melzack, Eds., pp. 186–194, Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, UK, 2nd edition, 1984.
[23]
A. J. Barsky, J. D. Goodson, R. S. Lane, and P. D. Cleary, “The amplification of somatic symptoms,” Psychosomatic Medicine, vol. 50, no. 5, pp. 510–519, 1988.
[24]
A. J. Barsky, G. Wyshak, and G. L. Klerman, “The somatosensory amplification scale and its relationship to hypochondriasis,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 323–334, 1990.
[25]
G. de Benedittis, R. Massei, R. Nobili, and A. Pieri, “The Italian pain questionnaire,” Pain, vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 53–62, 1988.
[26]
R. Melzack and W. S. Torgerson, “On the language of pain,” Anesthesiology, vol. 34, no. 1, pp. 50–59, 1971.
[27]
M. P. Jensen and P. Karoly, “Pain-specific beliefs, perceived symptom severity, and adjustment to chronic pain,” Clinical Journal of Pain, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 123–130, 1992.
[28]
I. Pilowsky and N. D. Spence, Manual for the Illness Behaviour Questionnaire (IBQ), University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia, 2nd edition, 1983.
[29]
L. R. Derogatis, R. S. Lipman, K. Rickels, E. H. Uhlenhuth, and L. Covi, “The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self report symptom inventory,” Behavioral Science, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 1–15, 1974.
[30]
A. Prunas, I. Sarno, E. Preti, F. Madeddu, and M. Perugini, “Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the SCL-90-R: a study on a large community sample,” European Psychiatry, vol. 27, no. 8, pp. 591–597, 2012.
[31]
R. D. Kerns, D. C. Turk, and T. E. Rudy, “The West Haven-Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI),” Pain, vol. 23, no. 4, pp. 345–356, 1985.
[32]
R. Ferrari, C. Novara, E. Sanavio, and F. Zerbini, “Internal structure and validity of the multidimensional pain inventory, Italian language version,” Pain Medicine, vol. 1, no. 2, pp. 123–130, 2000.
[33]
L. Colloca and F. G. Miller, “Harnessing the placebo effect: the need for translational research,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 366, no. 1572, pp. 1922–1930, 2011.
[34]
A. J. Barsky, H. M. Peekna, and J. F. Borus, “Somatic symptom reporting in women and men,” Journal of General Internal Medicine, vol. 16, no. 4, pp. 266–275, 2001.
[35]
F. H. Creed, I. Davies, J. Jackson et al., “The epidemiology of multiple somatic symptoms,” Journal of Psychosomatic Research, vol. 72, no. 4, pp. 311–317, 2012.