In the last decade, overt attention under natural conditions became a prominent topic in neuroscientific and psychological research. In this context, one central question is “what guides the direction of gaze on complex visual scenes?” In the present review recent research on bottom-up influences on overt attention is presented first. Against this background, strengths and limitations of the bottom-up approach are discussed and future directions in this field are outlined. In addition to that, the current scope on top-down factors in visual attention is enlarged by discussing the impact of emotions and motivational tendencies on viewing behavior. Overall, this review highlights how behavioral and neurophysiological research on overt attention can benefit from a broader scope on influential factors in visual attention. 1. Introduction Vision is the primary perceptual modality. Hence, visual information plays a key role in how we perceive the world and it constitutes the perceptual basis of our actions. The main task of the selection system is to choose the optimal location for the attention focus. This selection process is mediated by attention that bundles our limited cognitive resources and mediates an in-depth processing of environmental features at a given point in time [1, 2]. In this sense Posner [3] described the attentional focus in terms of a spotlight that scans the environment. In fact, the main task for the visual and oculomotor system is to direct one’s gaze to the most important object in the visual field so that this object is projected to the fovea centralis providing sharp central vision. Given that the physiology of the human eye restricts the amount of relatively less noisy visual input at a certain point in time, one central question is “what guides the direction of gaze?” In this context it has to be pointed out that we commonly differentiate between several definitions and components of visual attention; on the one hand, we speak of sustained attention when we describe our readiness to detect rarely and abrupt occurring signals over prolonged periods of time [4]. By contrast, our ability to attend to one visual signal of current importance while ignoring others is defined as selective attention [5]. Moreover, we can also divide our attention to different locations in the visual field even when locations are spatially separated [6, 7]. Different models have been proposed which incorporate these different components of attentional processes [8, 9] as well as further components such as an alerting system that is involved in preparing
References
[1]
M. M. Chun, J. D. Golomb, and N. B. Turk-Browne, “A Taxonomy of external and internal attention,” Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 62, pp. 73–101, 2011.
[2]
K. Kaspar and P. Konig, “Emotions and personality traits as high-level factors in visual attention,” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, vol. 6, article 321, pp. 1–14, 2012.
[3]
M. I. Posner, “Orienting of attention,” The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 3–25, 1980.
[4]
M. Sarter, B. Givens, and J. P. Bruno, “The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 35, no. 2, pp. 146–160, 2001.
[5]
C. M. Portas, G. Rees, A. M. Howseman, O. Josephs, R. Turner, and C. D. Frith, “A specific role for the thalamus in mediating the interaction of attention and arousal in humans,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 18, no. 21, pp. 8979–8989, 1998.
[6]
S. Hahn and A. F. Kramer, “Further evidence for the division of attention among non-contiguous locations,” Visual Cognition, vol. 5, no. 1-2, pp. 217–256, 1998.
[7]
M. M. Müller, P. Malinowski, T. Gruber, and S. A. Hillyard, “Sustained division of the attentional spotlight,” Nature, vol. 424, no. 6946, pp. 309–312, 2003.
[8]
M. Corbetta and G. L. Shulman, “Two attentional networks: identification and function within a larger cognitive architecture,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention, M. I. Posner, Ed., pp. 113–128, The Guilford Press, New York, NY, USA, 2012.
[9]
J. T. Coull, “Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology,” Progress in Neurobiology, vol. 55, no. 4, pp. 343–361, 1998.
[10]
M. I. Posner and S. E. Petersen, “The attention system of the human brain,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 13, pp. 25–42, 1990.
[11]
J. E. Hoffman and B. Subramaniam, “The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements,” Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 57, no. 6, pp. 787–795, 1995.
[12]
K. Kaspar and P. K?nig, “Viewing behavior and the impact of low-level image properties across repeated presentations of complex scenes,” Journal of Vision, vol. 11, no. 13, pp. 1–29, 2011.
[13]
I. Biederman, “Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding,” Psychological Review, vol. 94, no. 2, pp. 115–147, 1987.
[14]
A. K. Engel, P. Fries, and W. Singer, “Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 10, pp. 704–716, 2001.
[15]
C. Koch and S. Ullman, “Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry,” Human Neurobiology, vol. 4, no. 4, pp. 219–227, 1985.
[16]
L. Itti and C. Koch, “Computational modelling of visual attention,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 194–203, 2001.
[17]
S. M. Zeki, “Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey,” Nature, vol. 274, no. 5670, pp. 423–428, 1978.
[18]
J. Harel, C. Koch, and P. Perona, “Graph-based visual saliency,” in Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 19, B. Scholkopf, J. Platt, and T. Hoffman, Eds., pp. 545–552, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 2007.
[19]
T. Foulsham and G. Underwood, “What can saliency models predict about eye movements? Spatial and sequential aspects of fixations during encoding and recognition,” Journal of Vision, vol. 8, no. 2, article 6, 2008.
[20]
D. Parkhurst, K. Law, and E. Niebur, “Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention,” Vision Research, vol. 42, no. 1, pp. 107–123, 2002.
[21]
A. R. Koene and L. Zhaoping, “Feature-specific interactions in salience from combined feature contrasts: evidence for a bottom-up saliency map in V1,” Journal of Vision, vol. 7, no. 7, article 6, 2007.
[22]
S. K. Mannan, K. H. Ruddock, and D. S. Wooding, “Fixation patterns made during brief examination of two-dimensional images,” Perception, vol. 26, no. 8, pp. 1059–1072, 1997.
[23]
G. Krieger, I. Rentschler, G. Hauske, K. Schill, and C. Zetzsche, “Object and scene analysis by saccadic eye-movements: an investigation with higher-order statistics,” Spatial Vision, vol. 13, no. 2-3, pp. 201–214, 2000.
[24]
W. Einh?user and P. K?nig, “Does luminance-contrast contribute to a saliency map for overt visual attention?” European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 17, no. 5, pp. 1089–1097, 2003.
[25]
R. J. Baddeley and B. W. Tatler, “High frequency edges (but not contrast) predict where we fixate: a Bayesian system identification analysis,” Vision Research, vol. 46, no. 18, pp. 2824–2833, 2006.
[26]
H.-P. Frey, P. K?nig, and W. Einh?user, “The role of first- and second-order stimulus features for human overt attention,” Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 69, no. 2, pp. 153–161, 2007.
[27]
G. Underwood, T. Foulsham, and K. Humphrey, “Saliency and scan patterns in the inspection of real-world scenes: eye movements during encoding and recognition,” Visual Cognition, vol. 17, no. 6-7, pp. 812–834, 2009.
[28]
W. Einh?user, M. Spain, and P. Perona, “Objects predict fixations better than early saliency,” Journal of Vision, vol. 8, no. 14, article 18, 2008.
[29]
M. Nystrom and K. Holmqvist, “Semantic override of low-level features in image viewing—both initially and overall,” Journal of Eye Movement Research, vol. 2, pp. 1–11, 2008.
[30]
B. W. Tatler, R. J. Baddeley, and I. D. Gilchrist, “Visual correlates of fixation selection: effects of scale and time,” Vision Research, vol. 45, no. 5, pp. 643–659, 2005.
[31]
J. M. Henderson, J. R. Brockmole, M. S. Castelhano, and M. Mack, “Visual saliency does not account for eye movements during search in real-world scenes,” in Eye Movements: A Window on Mind and Brain, R. van Gompel, M. Fischer, W. Murray, and R. Hill, Eds., pp. 537–562, Elsevier, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
[32]
B. W. Tatler, “The central fixation bias in scene viewing: selecting an optimal viewing position independently of motor biases and image feature distributions,” Journal of Vision, vol. 7, no. 14, article 4, 2007.
[33]
S. Yantis and J. Jonides, “Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol. 10, no. 5, pp. 601–621, 1984.
[34]
A. M. Treisman and G. Gelade, “A feature-integration theory of attention,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 97–136, 1980.
[35]
K. Kaspar and P. K?nig, “Overt attention and context factors: the impact of repeated presentations, image type, and individual motivation,” PLoS ONE, vol. 6, no. 7, Article ID e21719, 2011.
[36]
B. W. Tatler, R. J. Baddeley, and B. T. Vincent, “The long and the short of it: spatial statistics at fixation vary with saccade amplitude and task,” Vision Research, vol. 46, no. 12, pp. 1857–1862, 2006.
[37]
A. Acik, A. Sarwary, R. Schultze-Kraft, S. Onat, and P. Konig, “Developmental changes in natural viewing behavior: bottom-up and top-down differences between children, young adults and older adults,” Frontiers in Psychology, vol. 1, article 207, 2010.
[38]
Q. Zhao and C. Koch, “Learning a saliency map using fixated locations in natural scenes,” Journal of Vision, vol. 11, no. 3, pp. 1–5, 2011.
[39]
A. Soltani and C. Koch, “Visual saliency computations: mechanisms, constraints, and the effect of feedback,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 30, no. 38, pp. 12831–12843, 2010.
[40]
J. D. Schall and K. G. Thompson, “Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 22, pp. 241–259, 1999.
[41]
J. P. Gottlieb, M. Kusunoki, and M. E. Goldberg, “The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex,” Nature, vol. 391, no. 6666, pp. 481–484, 1998.
[42]
M. S. Livingstone and D. H. Hubel, “Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 7, no. 11, pp. 3416–3468, 1987.
[43]
B. E. Burrows and T. Moore, “Influence and limitations of popout in the selection of salient visual stimuli by area V4 neurons,” Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 29, no. 48, pp. 15169–15177, 2009.
[44]
D. Mahapatra, S. Winkler, and S.-C. Yen, “Motion saliency outweighs other low-level features while watching videos,” in Human Vision and Electronic Imaging XIII, vol. 6806 of Proceedings of SPIE, pp. 1–10, January 2008.
[45]
Y.-F. Ma, L. Lu, H.-J. Zhang, and M. Li, “A user attention model for video summarization,” in Proceedings of the 10th ACM International Conference of Multimedia, pp. 533–542, December 2002.
[46]
K. Kaspar, U. Hassler, U. Martens, N. Trujillo-Barreto, and T. Gruber, “Steady-state visually evoked potential correlates of object recognition,” Brain Research, vol. 1343, pp. 112–121, 2010.
[47]
A. Torralba and A. Oliva, “Statistics of natural image categories,” Network: Computation in Neural Systems, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 391–412, 2003.
[48]
L. Velisavljevic and J. H. Elder, “What do we see in a glance?” Journal of Vision, vol. 2, no. 7, p. 493, 2002.
[49]
J. D. Eastwood, D. Smilek, and P. M. Merikle, “Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion,” Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 63, no. 6, pp. 1004–1013, 2001.
[50]
D. Lamy, L. Amunts, and Y. Bar-Haim, “Emotional priming of pop-out in visual search,” Emotion, vol. 8, no. 2, pp. 151–161, 2008.
[51]
J. Triesch, D. H. Ballard, M. M. Hayhoe, and B. T. Sullivan, “What you see is what you need,” Journal of Vision, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 86–94, 2003.
[52]
C. A. Rothkopf, D. H. Ballard, and M. M. Hayhoe, “Task and context determine where you look,” Journal of Vision, vol. 7, no. 14, article 16, 2007.
[53]
K.-C. Hamborg, M. Bruns, F. Ollermann, and K. Kaspar, “The effect of banner animation on fixation behavior and recall performance in search tasks,” Computers in Human Behavior, vol. 28, no. 2, pp. 576–582, 2012.
[54]
T. Betz, T. C. Kietzmann, N. Wilming, and P. K?nig, “Investigating task-dependent top-down effects on overt visual attention,” Journal of Vision, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 1–14, 2010.
[55]
E. A. Kensinger, “Remembering emotional experiences: the contribution of valence and arousal,” Reviews in the Neurosciences, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 241–251, 2004.
[56]
R. D. Lane, P. M.-L. Chua, and R. J. Dolan, “Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 37, no. 9, pp. 989–997, 1999.
[57]
P. J. Lang, “The varieties of emotional experience: a meditation on James-Lange theory,” Psychological Review, vol. 101, no. 2, pp. 211–221, 1994.
[58]
H. J. Eysenck, “General features of the model,” in A Model for Personality, H. J. Eysenck, Ed., pp. 1–37, Springer, New York, NY, USA, 1981.
[59]
J. A. Gray, “A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality,” in A Model for Personality, H. J. Eysenck, Ed., pp. 246–276, Springer, New York, NY, USA, 1981.
[60]
D. Derryberry and M. K. Rothbart, “Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 55, no. 6, pp. 958–966, 1988.
[61]
K. Kaspar, T. M. Hloucal, J. Kriz et al., “Emotions' impact on viewing behavior under natural conditions,” PloS ONE, vol. 8, Article ID e52737, 2013.
[62]
J. Heckhausen and H. Heckhausen, Motivation und Handeln, Springer, Heidelberg, Germany, 2006.
[63]
J. F. Rauthmann, C. T. Seubert, P. Sachse, and M. R. Furtner, “Eyes as windows to the soul: gazing behavior is related to personality,” Journal of Research in Personality, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 147–156, 2012.
[64]
H. Fischer, G. Wik, and M. Fredrikson, “Extraversion, neuroticism and brain function: a pet study of personality,” Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 345–352, 1997.
[65]
B. A. Steinberg and J. R. Augustine, “Behavioral, anatomical, and physiological aspects of recovery of motor function following stroke,” Brain Research Reviews, vol. 25, no. 1, pp. 125–132, 1997.
[66]
D. A. Cobb-Clark and S. Schurer, “The stability of big-five personality traits,” Economics Letters, vol. 115, no. 1, pp. 11–15, 2012.
[67]
M. B. Donnellan and R. E. Lucas, “Age differences in the big five across the life span: evidence from two national samples,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 558–566, 2008.