全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Phonological Fluency Strategy of Switching Differentiates Relapsing-Remitting and Secondary Progressive Multiple Sclerosis Patients

DOI: 10.1155/2013/451429

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

The strategies used to perform a verbal fluency task appear to be reflective of cognitive abilities necessary for successful daily functioning. In the present study, we explored potential differences in verbal fluency strategies (switching and clustering) used to maximize word production by patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS) versus patients with secondary progressive multiple sclerosis (SPMS). We further assessed impairment rates and potential differences in the sensitivity and specificity of phonological versus semantic verbal fluency tasks in discriminating between those with a diagnosis of MS and healthy adults. We found that the overall rate of impaired verbal fluency in our MS sample was consistent with that in other studies. However, we found no differences between types of MS (SPMS, RRMS), on semantic or phonological fluency word production, or the strategies used to maximize semantic fluency. In contrast, we found that the number of switches differed significantly in the phonological fluency task between the SPMS and RRMS subtypes. The clinical utility of semantic versus phonological fluency in discriminating MS patients from healthy controls did not indicate any significant differences. Further, the strategies used to maximize performance did not differentiate MS subgroups or MS patients from healthy controls. 1. Introduction Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic, debilitating, autoimmune disease of the nervous system that usually presents with a relapsing-remitting, and then later a progressive, course. Both the course of the illness and the presentation of motor and cognitive symptoms in terms of type and severity can vary significantly from one individual to another [1]. The disease has been classified as a frontal-subcortical dementia, as it causes demyelination of neurons mainly in frontal and subcortical regions [2]. It has been estimated that approximately 60% of patients with multiple sclerosis present with cognitive deficits [3]. Consistent with the locations of disease-induced lesions, subsequent functional impairments comprise problems with attention, information processing speed, memory, and executive functioning, all of which limit the individual’s ability to perform within the context of work and social relationships and may even compromise the safety of daily activities such as driving [4]. Although the cognitive effects of MS have been repeatedly documented in previous studies [3], it is not clear whether cognitive deficits are exclusively the result of neurological damage or could also be the product of

References

[1]  H. L. Weiner, “The challenge of multiple sclerosis: how do we cure a chronic heterogeneous disease?” Annals of Neurology, vol. 65, no. 3, pp. 239–248, 2009.
[2]  R. M. Bonelli and J. L. Cummings, “Frontal-subcortical dementias,” Neurologist, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 100–107, 2008.
[3]  M. P. Amato, G. Ponziani, G. Siracusa, and S. Sorbi, “Cognitive dysfunction in early-onset multiple sclerosis: a reappraisal after 10 years,” Archives of Neurology, vol. 58, no. 10, pp. 1602–1606, 2001.
[4]  K. A. Ryan, L. J. Rapport, K. T. Harper et al., “Fitness to drive in multiple sclerosis: awareness of deficit moderates risk,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 31, no. 1, pp. 126–139, 2009.
[5]  B. J. Diamond, S. K. Johnson, M. Kaufman, and L. Graves, “Relationships between information processing, depression, fatigue and cognition in multiple sclerosis,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 189–199, 2008.
[6]  C. Potagas, E. Giogkaraki, G. Koutsis et al., “Cognitive impairment in different MS subtypes and clinically isolated syndromes,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 267, no. 1-2, pp. 100–106, 2008.
[7]  U. Wachowius, M. Talley, N. Silver, H. J. Heinze, and M. Sailer, “Cognitive impairment in primary and secondary progressive multiple sclerosis,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 27, no. 1, pp. 65–77, 2005.
[8]  J. Razani, R. Casas, J. T. Wong, P. Lu, C. Alessi, and K. Josephson, “Relationship between executive functioning and activities of daily living in patients with relatively mild dementia,” Applied Neuropsychology, vol. 14, no. 3, pp. 208–214, 2007.
[9]  T. Nemoto, H. Kashima, and M. Mizuno, “Contribution of divergent thinking to community functioning in schizophrenia,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, vol. 31, no. 2, pp. 517–524, 2007.
[10]  J. D. Henry and W. W. Beatty, “Verbal fluency deficits in multiple sclerosis,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 44, no. 7, pp. 1166–1174, 2006.
[11]  A. I. Tr?ster, J. A. Fields, J. A. Testa et al., “Cortical and subcortical influences on clustering and switching in the performance of verbal fluency tasks,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 295–304, 1998.
[12]  W. I. McDonald, A. Compston, G. Edan et al., “Recommended diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: guidelines from the International Panel on the Diagnosis of Multiple Sclerosis,” Annals of Neurology, vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 121–127, 2001.
[13]  J. F. Kurtzke, “Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS),” Neurology, vol. 33, no. 11, pp. 1444–1452, 1983.
[14]  A. T. Beck, R. A. Steer, Brown, and GK, BDI: Fast Screen for Medical Patients, TX: Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, Tex, USA, 2000.
[15]  K. N. Fountoulakis, M. Tsolaki, H. Chantzi, and A. Kazis, “Mini mental state examination (MMSE): a validation study in Greece,” American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, vol. 15, no. 6, pp. 342–345, 2000.
[16]  M. H. Kosmidis, C. H. Vlahou, P. Panagiotaki, and G. Kiosseoglou, “The verbal fluency task in the Greek population: normative data, and clustering and switching strategies,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 164–172, 2004.
[17]  K. K. Zakzanis, “Distinct neurocognitive profiles in multiple sclerosis subtypes,” Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, vol. 15, no. 2, pp. 115–136, 2000.
[18]  S. C. J. Huijbregts, N. F. Kalkers, L. M. J. De Sonneville, V. De Groot, I. E. W. Reuling, and C. H. Polman, “Differences in cognitive impairment of relapsing remitting, secondary, and primary progressive MS,” Neurology, vol. 63, no. 2, pp. 335–339, 2004.
[19]  W. W. Beatty, “Fluency in multiple sclerosis: which measure is best?” Multiple Sclerosis, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 261–264, 2002.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133