全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

The Role of Enactment in Learning American Sign Language in Younger and Older Adults

DOI: 10.1155/2013/285860

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

“Tell me, and I will forget. Show me, and I may remember. Involve me, and I will understand” (Confucius, 450 B.C). Philosophers and scientists alike have pondered the question of the mind-body link for centuries. Recently the role of motor information has been examined more specifically for a role in learning and memory. This paper describes a study using an errorless learning protocol to teach characters to young and older persons in American Sign Language. Participants were assigned to one of two groups: recognition (visually recognizing signs) or enactment (physically creating signs). Number of signs recalled and rate of forgetting were compared between groups and across age cohorts. There were no significant differences, within either the younger or older groups for number of items recalled. There were significant differences between recognition and enactment groups for rate of forgetting, within young and old, suggesting that enactment improves the strength of memory for items learned, regardless of age. 1. Introduction A growing volume of research is dedicated to understanding the role of physical engagement in learning, memory, and retrieval; however, this is by no means a new field of inquiry. Herman Ebbinghaus [1], a pioneer in the study of learning and memory, proposed a model that plotted the rate of forgetting and the factors that influenced the process of forgetting. Memory strength was dependent on two factors in his model: memory representation and active recall. Memory representation can be improved through environmental enrichment, which involved adding sight, sound, scent and tactile input to enhance the memory representation for an item. Active recall, or spaced repetition, improves memory strength by increasing time between recall so that the memory trace is being strengthened with each recall attempt [2, 3]. Environmental enrichment, the first component of Ebbinghaus’s memory strength model, can include visual, auditory, olfactory, and tactile stimulation to engage multimodal processing. Enactment engages tactile and kinesthetic feedback by pairing a physical activity or movement to an item to be remembered. Research has consistently shown that enactment of verbal items provides enhanced memory performance compared to traditional verbal recall tasks [4]. An example of enactment of verbal items would be bringing your hand to your mouth to mime eating, when the spoon or fork is the verbal item to be remembered. There are four main theories in the literature proposed to explain the enactment effect (also referred to as the subject

References

[1]  H. Ebbinghaus, Memory: A Contribution to experimental Psychology, Dover, Mineola, NY, USA, 1964.
[2]  J. D. Karpicke and J. R. Blunt, “Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping,” Science, vol. 331, no. 6018, pp. 772–775, 2011.
[3]  N. Kornell, A. D. Castel, T. S. Eich, and R. A. Bjork, “Spacing as the friend of both memory and induction in young and older adults,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 498–503, 2010.
[4]  J. D. Von Essen and L. G. Nilsson, “Memory effects of motor activation in subject-performed tasks and sign language,” Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, vol. 10, no. 2, pp. 445–449, 2003.
[5]  R. L. Cohen and G. Bean, “Memory in educable mentally retarded adults: Deficit in subject or experimenter?” Intelligence, vol. 7, no. 3, pp. 287–298, 1983.
[6]  L. Nilsson, “Remembering Actions and Words,” in Oxford Handbook of Memory, E. Tulving and F. I. M. Craik, Eds., pp. 137–148, Oxford Press, New York. NY, USA, 2000.
[7]  L. B?ckman and L. G. Nilsson, “Aging effects in free recall: an exception to the rule,” Human Learning, vol. 3, pp. 53–69, 1984.
[8]  L. Backman and L. G. Nilsson, “Prerequisites for lack of age differences in memory performance,” Experimental Aging Research, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 67–73, 1985.
[9]  E. Tulving and F. I. M. Craik, Eds., The Oxford Handbook of Memory, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2005.
[10]  R. Kormi-Nouri, “The nature of memory for action events: an episodic integration view,” European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, vol. 7, pp. 337–363, 1995.
[11]  P. Feyereisen, “Enactment effects and integration processes in younger and older adults' memory for actions,” Memory, vol. 17, no. 4, pp. 374–385, 2009.
[12]  D. A. Balota, J. Duchek, and J. Logan, “Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval?” in The Foundations of Remembering, J. S. Nairne, Ed., Psychology Press, Florence, Ky, USA, 2007.
[13]  D. A. Balota, J. M. Duchek, S. D. Sergent-Marshall, and H. L. Roediger, “Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal-interval spacing? Explorations of spacing effects in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer's disease,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 19–31, 2006.
[14]  J. M. Logan and D. A. Balota, “Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: Exploring different schedules of spacing and retention interval in younger and older adults,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 257–280, 2008.
[15]  C. P. Rea and V. Modigliani, “The spacing effect in 4- to 9-year-old children,” Memory and Cognition, vol. 15, no. 5, pp. 436–443, 1987.
[16]  C. J. Camp, J. W. Foss, A. M. O'Hanlon, and A. B. Stevens, “Memory interventions for persons with dementia,” Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 193–210, 1996.
[17]  C. J. Camp and M. J. Bird, “Retrieval strategies as a rehabilitation aid for cognitive loss in pathological aging,” in Cognitive Rehabilitation in Old Age, R. D. Hill, L. Backman, and A. S. Neely, Eds., pp. 224–228, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2000.
[18]  A. K. Hochhalter, J. B. Overmier, S. M. Gasper, B. L. Bakke, and R. J. Holub, “A comparison of spaced retrieval to other schedules of practice for people with dementia,” Experimental Aging Research, vol. 31, no. 2, pp. 101–118, 2005.
[19]  L. Fetters, “Perspective on variability in the development of human action,” Physical Therapy, vol. 90, no. 12, pp. 1860–1867, 2010.
[20]  A. Baddeley and B. A. Wilson, “When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination,” Neuropsychologia, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 53–68, 1994.
[21]  M. L?vdén, M. R?nnlund, and L. G. Nilsson, “Remembering and knowing in adulthood: Effects of enacted encoding and relations to processing speed,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 184–200, 2002.
[22]  E. Tulving and D. M. Thomson, “Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory,” PSYCHOL.REV., vol. 80, no. 5, pp. 352–373, 1973.
[23]  D. C. Park and P. Reuter-Lorenz, “The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding,” Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 60, pp. 173–196, 2009.
[24]  S. F. Witelson, “Hemispheric specialization for linguistic and nonlinguistic tactual perception using a dichotomous stimulation technique,” Cortex, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 3–17, 1974.
[25]  F. M. Wolf, Meta-Analysis: Quantitative Methods for Research Synthesis, Sage, Beverly Hills, Calif, USA, 1986.
[26]  N. W. Mulligan and S. L. Hornstein, “Memory for actions: Self-performed tasks and the reenactment effect,” Memory and Cognition, vol. 31, no. 3, pp. 412–421, 2003.
[27]  J. Jeka, K. S. Oie, and T. Kiemel, “Multisensory information for human postural control: Integrating touch and vision,” Experimental Brain Research, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 107–125, 2000.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133