Psychotic disorders carry social and economic costs for sufferers and society. Recent evidence highlights the risk posed by urban upbringing and social deprivation in the genesis of paranoia and psychosis. Evidence based psychological interventions are often not offered because of a lack of therapists. Virtual reality (VR) environments have been used to treat mental health problems. VR may be a way of understanding the aetiological processes in psychosis and increasing psychotherapeutic resources for its treatment. We developed a high-fidelity virtual reality scenario of an urban street scene to test the hypothesis that virtual urban exposure is able to generate paranoia to a comparable or greater extent than scenarios using indoor scenes. Participants ( ) entered the VR scenario for four minutes, after which time their degree of paranoid ideation was assessed. We demonstrated that the virtual reality scenario was able to elicit paranoia in a nonclinical, healthy group and that an urban scene was more likely to lead to higher levels of paranoia than a virtual indoor environment. We suggest that this study offers evidence to support the role of exposure to factors in the urban environment in the genesis and maintenance of psychotic experiences and symptoms. The realistic high-fidelity street scene scenario may offer a useful tool for therapists. 1. Introduction Psychosis, and psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia, is characterized by the presence of hallucinations (false perceptions) and delusions (false beliefs). It has become clear in recent years that the marked heterogeneity in the rates of schizophrenia and psychotic disorders across population groups [1] can be partly explained by urban birth and upbringing, migration, ethnicity, and what Cantor-Graae and Selten have termed “social defeat” [2, 3]. A particularly important recent body of research is the MRC AESOP study that demonstrated a twentyfold rate increase in the incidence of psychosis in London, compared with Nottingham and Bristol, and the very highest rates being within the Black and ethnic minority groups [4–8]. These epidemiological findings have strengthened continuum models of psychosis [9], which state that psychotic-like experiences are distributed in a normal nonclinical population and might be dependent upon many of the same variables as causally responsible for cases of the disorder [10]. Trying to connect psychological, biological, and social models of psychosis, and relating these to psychotic experiences and urbanicity in the general population, has become increasingly
References
[1]
J. J. McGrath, “The surprisingly rich contours of schizophrenia epidemiology,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 64, no. 1, pp. 14–16, 2007.
[2]
E. Cantor-Graae and J. Selten, “Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 67, no. 1, suupplement 1, article 63, 2004.
[3]
J.-P. Selten and E. Cantor-Graae, “Social defeat: risk factor for schizophrenia?” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 187, pp. 101–102, 2005.
[4]
P. Fearon, J. B. Kirkbride, C. Morgan, et al., “Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in ethnic minority groups: results from the MRC AESOP Study,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 36, no. 11, pp. 1541–1550, 2006.
[5]
P. Fearon and C. Morgan, “Environmental factors in schizophrenia: the role of migrant studies,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, no. 3, pp. 405–408, 2006.
[6]
J. B. Kirkbride, P. Fearon, C. Morgan et al., “Neighbourhood variation in the incidence of psychotic disorders in Southeast London,” Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 438–445, 2007.
[7]
J. B. Kirkbride, P. Fearon, C. Morgan et al., “Heterogeneity in incidence rates of schizophrenia and other psychotic syndromes: findings from the 3-center AESOP study,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 63, no. 3, pp. 250–258, 2006.
[8]
J. B. Kirkbride, C. Morgan, P. Fearon, P. Dazzan, R. M. Murray, and P. B. Jones, “Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 37, no. 10, pp. 1413–1425, 2007.
[9]
L. C. Johns and J. van Os, “The continuity of psychotic experiences in the general population,” Clinical Psychology Review, vol. 21, no. 8, pp. 1125–1141, 2001.
[10]
M. R. Broome, J. B. Woolley, P. Tabraham et al., “What causes the onset of psychosis?” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 79, no. 1, pp. 23–34, 2005.
[11]
P. A. Garety, P. Bebbington, D. Fowler, D. Freeman, and E. Kuipers, “Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis: a theoretical paper,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 37, no. 10, pp. 1377–1391, 2007.
[12]
I. Myin-Germeys, P. Delespaul, and J. van Os, “Behavioral sensitization to daily life stress in psychosis,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 35, no. 5, pp. 733–741, 2005.
[13]
I. Myin-Germeys, L. Krabbendam, P. Delespaul, and J. van Os, “Can cognitive deficits explain differential sensitivity to life events in psychosis?” Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, vol. 38, no. 5, pp. 262–268, 2003.
[14]
I. Myin-Germeys, L. Krabbendam, P. A. E. G. Delespaul, and J. van Os, “Do life events have their effect on psychosis by influencing the emotional reactivity to daily life stress?” Psychological Medicine, vol. 33, no. 2, pp. 327–333, 2003.
[15]
I. Myin-Germeys, F. Peeters, R. Havermans et al., “Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: an experience sampling study,” Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, vol. 107, no. 2, pp. 124–131, 2003.
[16]
I. Myin-Germeys, J. van Os, J. E. Schwartz, A. A. Stone, and P. A. Delespaul, “Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 58, no. 12, pp. 1137–1144, 2001.
[17]
L. Ellett, D. Freeman, and P. A. Garety, “The psychological effect of an urban environment on individuals with persecutory delusions: the Camberwell walk study,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 99, no. 1, pp. 77–84, 2008.
[18]
D. Freeman, M. Slater, P. E. Bebbington et al., “Can virtual reality be used to investigate persecutory ideation?” Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, vol. 191, no. 8, pp. 509–514, 2003.
[19]
L. R. Valmaggia, D. Freeman, C. Green et al., “Virtual reality and paranoid ideations in people with an 'at-risk mental state' for psychosis,” The British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 191, no. 51, pp. s63–s68, 2007.
[20]
D. Freeman, K. Pugh, A. Antley et al., “Virtual reality study of paranoid thinking in the general population,” The British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 192, no. 4, pp. 258–263, 2008.
[21]
E. Zányi, E. Selmanovic, M. Broome, et al., “Interactive highly realistic virtual reality as a tool for understanding the genesis and treatment of psychotic symptoms,” Studies in Health Technology and Informatics, vol. 144, pp. 138–140, 2009.
[22]
C. E. L. Green, D. Freeman, E. Kuipers et al., “Measuring ideas of persecution and social reference: the Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scales (GPTS),” Psychological Medicine, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 101–111, 2008.
[23]
V. Bell, P. W. Halligan, and H. D. Ellis, “The Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS): a new validated measure of anomalous perceptual experience,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 366–377, 2006.
[24]
D. Watson and R. Friend, “Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety,” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 448–457, 1969.
[25]
T. J. Meyer, M. L. Miller, R. L. Metzger, and T. D. Borkovec, “Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 28, no. 6, pp. 487–495, 1990.
[26]
P. Boyce and G. Parker, “Development of a scale to measure interpersonal sensitivity,” Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 341–351, 1989.
[27]
D. Freeman, K. Pugh, C. Green, L. Valmaggia, G. Dunn, and P. Garety, “A measure of state persecutory ideation for experimental studies,” Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, vol. 195, no. 9, pp. 781–784, 2007.
[28]
M. R. Broome, P. Wuyts, and P. Fusar-Poli, “Philosophical and ethical issues in the prodromal phase of psychosis,” in Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Psychiatry, K. W. M. Fulford, M. Davies, G. Graham, J. Sadler, G. Stanghellini, and T. Thornton, Eds., Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2013.
[29]
M. R. Broome, F. Day, I. Valli et al., “Delusional ideation, manic symptomatology and working memory in a cohort at clinical high-risk for psychosis:a longitudinal study,” European Psychiatry, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 258–263, 2012.
[30]
M. Broome and P. Fusar-Poli, “Philosophical issues in the prodromal phase of psychosis,” Current Pharmaceutical Design, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 596–605, 2012.
[31]
L. Bortolotti and M. R. Broome, “Affective dimensions of the phenomenon of double bookkeeping in delusions,” Emotion Review, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 187–191, 2012.