Cognitive deficits across a wide range of domains have been consistently observed in schizophrenia and are linked to poor functional outcome (Green, 1996; Carter, 2006). Language abnormalities are among the most salient and include disorganized speech as well as deficits in comprehension. In this review, we aim to evaluate impairments of language processing in schizophrenia in relation to a domain-general control deficit. We first provide an overview of language comprehension in the healthy human brain, stressing the role of cognitive control processes, especially during discourse comprehension. We then discuss cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia, before turning to evidence suggesting that schizophrenia patients are particularly impaired at processing meaningful discourse as a result of deficits in control functions. We conclude that domain-general control mechanisms are impaired in schizophrenia and that during language comprehension this is most likely to result in difficulties during the processing of discourse-level context, which involves integrating and maintaining multiple levels of meaning. Finally, we predict that language comprehension in schizophrenia patients will be most impaired during discourse processing. We further suggest that discourse comprehension problems in schizophrenia might be mitigated when conflicting information is absent and strong relations amongst individual words are present in the discourse context. “There is no “centre of Speech” in the brain any more than there is a faculty of Speech in the mind. The entire brain, more or less, is at work in a man who uses language” William James 1. Introduction Impaired cognition across a wide range of cognitive domains is a pervasive feature of schizophrenia and is connected to poor functional outcome for patients [1, 2]. Of the cognitive deficits that have been observed in schizophrenia patients, language abnormalities are among the most salient and include disorganized speech as well as deficits in comprehension. However, there is no general consensus as to whether the cognitive impairments seen in schizophrenia can be attributed to a single disrupted mechanism, to multiple disrupted systems, or to low-level perceptual deficits. Accounts of language deficits in schizophrenia might be divided into theories that focus on irregularities in semantic memory structure and functioning, and those that emphasize deficits in the ability to effectively use context [3]. Accounts that posit abnormalities in semantic memory in schizophrenia, such as the exaggerated spread of activation
References
[1]
M. F. Green, “What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 153, no. 3, pp. 321–330, 1996.
[2]
C. S. Carter, “Understanding the glass ceiling for functional outcome in schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 163, no. 3, pp. 356–358, 2006.
[3]
G. R. Kuperberg, D. A. Kreher, and T. Ditman, “What can event-related potentials tell us about language, and perhaps even thought, in schizophrenia?” International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 75, no. 2, pp. 66–76, 2010.
[4]
M. Spitzer, U. Braun, L. Hermle, and S. Maier, “Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: direct evidence from indirect semantic priming,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 34, no. 12, pp. 864–877, 1993.
[5]
M. A. Gernsbacher, K. A. Tallent, and C. M. Bolliger, “Disordered discourse in schizophrenia described by the structure building framework,” Discourse Studies, vol. 1, no. 3, pp. 355–372, 1999.
[6]
G. R. Kuperberg, “Language in schizophrenia part 2: what can psycholinguistics bring to the study of schizophrenia and vice versa?” Linguistics and Language Compass, vol. 4, no. 8, pp. 590–604, 2010.
[7]
G. R. Kuperberg, T. Deckersbach, D. J. Holt, D. Goff, and W. C. West, “Increased temporal and prefrontal activity in response to semantic associations in schizophrenia,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 64, no. 2, pp. 138–151, 2007.
[8]
M. A. Gernsbacher, “The structure-building framework: what it is, what it might also be, and why,” in Language Comprehension as Structure Building, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1990.
[9]
M. A. Gernsbacher and M. E. Faust, “The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 245–262, 1991.
[10]
M. A. Gernsbacher, “Two decades of structure building,” Discourse Processes, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 265–304, 1997.
[11]
P. Hagoort, “The memory, unification, and control (MUC) model of language,” in Automaticity and Control in Language Processing, A. S. Meyer, L.R. Wheeldon, and A. Krott, Eds., Psychology Press, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
[12]
J. M. Novick, J. C. Trueswell, and S. L. Thompson-Schill, “Cognitive control and parsing: reexamining the role of Broca's area in sentence comprehension,” Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 5, no. 3, pp. 263–281, 2005.
[13]
J. M. Novick, J. C. Trueswell, and S. L. Thompson-Schill, “Broca's area and language processing: evidence for the cognitive control connection,” Linguistics and Language Compass, vol. 4, no. 10, pp. 906–924, 2010.
[14]
E. K. Miller and J. D. Cohen, “An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function,” Annual Review of Neuroscience, vol. 24, pp. 167–202, 2001.
[15]
E. Callaway and S. Naghdi, “An information processing model for schizophrenia,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 39, no. 3, pp. 339–347, 1982.
[16]
D. L. Braff, “Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 19, no. 2, pp. 233–259, 1993.
[17]
J. D. Cohen and D. Servan-Schreiber, “Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia,” Psychological Review, vol. 99, no. 1, pp. 45–77, 1992.
[18]
T. A. Lesh, T. A. Niendam, M. J. Minzenberg, and C. S. Carter, “Cognitive control deficits in schizophrenia: mechanisms and meaning,” Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. 36, no. 1, pp. 316–338, 2011.
[19]
E. C. Dias, P. D. Butler, M. J. Hoptman, and D. C. Javitt, “Early sensory contributions to contextual encoding deficits in schizophrenia,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 68, no. 7, pp. 654–664, 2011.
[20]
M. J. Minzenberg, A. R. Laird, S. Thelen, C. S. Carter, and D. C. Glahn, “Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 66, no. 8, pp. 811–822, 2009.
[21]
A. W. MacDonald, C. S. Carter, J. G. Kerns et al., “Specificity of prefrontal dysfunction and context processing deficits to schizophrenia in never-medicated patients with first-episode psychosis,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 162, no. 3, pp. 475–484, 2005.
[22]
D. M. Barch, C. S. Carter, T. S. Braver et al., “Selective deficits in prefrontal cortex function in medication-naive patients with schizophrenia,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 58, no. 3, pp. 280–288, 2001.
[23]
W. M. Perlstein, C. S. Carter, D. C. Noll, and J. D. Cohen, “Relation of prefrontal cortex dysfunction to working memory and symptoms in schizophrenia,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 158, no. 7, pp. 1105–1113, 2001.
[24]
J. H. Yoon, M. J. Minzenberg, S. Ursu et al., “Association of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction with disrupted coordinated brain activity in schizophrenia: relationship with impaired cognition, behavioral disorganization, and global function,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 165, no. 8, pp. 1006–1014, 2008.
[25]
S. G. Potkin, J. A. Turner, G. G. Brown et al., “Working memory and DLPFC inefficiency in schizophrenia: the FBIRN study,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 19–31, 2009.
[26]
P. C. Fletcher, P. J. McKenna, C. D. Frith, P. M. Grasby, K. J. Friston, and R. J. Dolan, “Brain activations in schizophrenia during a graded memory task studied with functional neuroimaging,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 55, no. 11, pp. 1001–1008, 1998.
[27]
G. R. Kuperberg, T. Sitnikova, D. Caplan, and P. J. Holcomb, “Electrophysiological distinctions in processing conceptual relationships within simple sentences,” Cognitive Brain Research, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 117–129, 2003.
[28]
G. R. Kuperberg, “Language in schizophrenia part 1: an introduction,” Linguistics and Language Compass, vol. 4, no. 8, pp. 576–589, 2010.
[29]
N. C. Andreasen, “Thought, language, and communication disorders. I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 36, no. 12, pp. 1315–1321, 1979.
[30]
N. C. Andreasen, “Thought, language, and communication disorders. II. Diagnostic significance,” Archives of General Psychiatry, vol. 36, no. 12, pp. 1325–1330, 1979.
[31]
D. M. Barch, D. Servan-Schreiber, S. Steingard, J. D. Cohen, S. S. Steinhauer, and D. P. Van Kammen, “Semantic priming in schizophrenia: an examination of spreading activation using word pronunciation and multiple SOAs,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 105, no. 4, pp. 592–601, 1996.
[32]
D. A. Kreher, P. J. Holcomb, D. Goff, and G. R. Kuperberg, “Neural evidence for faster and further automatic spreading activation in schizophrenic thought disorder,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 473–482, 2008.
[33]
D. A. Kreher, D. Goff, and G. R. Kuperberg, “Why all the confusion? Experimental task explains discrepant semantic priming effects in schizophrenia under "automatic" conditions: evidence from event-related potentials,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 111, no. 1–3, pp. 174–181, 2009.
[34]
T. C. Manschreck, B. A. Maher, J. J. Milavetz, D. Ames, C. C. Weisstein, and M. L. Schneyer, “Semantic priming in thought disordered schizophrenic patients,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 61–66, 1988.
[35]
G. R. Kuperberg, P. K. McGuire, and A. S. David, “Reduced sensitivity to linguistic context in schizophrenic thought disorder: evidence from on-line monitoring for words in linguistically anomalous sentences,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 107, no. 3, pp. 423–434, 1998.
[36]
T. Sitnikova, D. F. Salisbury, G. Kuperberg, and P. J. Holcomb, “Electrophysiological insights into language processing in schizophrenia,” Psychophysiology, vol. 39, no. 6, pp. 851–860, 2002.
[37]
G. R. Kuperberg, T. Sitnikova, D. Goff, and P. J. Holcomb, “Making sense of sentences in schizophrenia: electrophysiological evidence for abnormal interactions between semantic and syntactic processing,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 115, no. 2, pp. 251–265, 2006.
[38]
T. Y. Swaab, K. Ledoux, C. C. Camblin, and M. Boudewyn, “ERPs and language processing,” in Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, S. J. Luck and E. S. Kappenman, Eds., chapter 14, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2011.
[39]
G. R. Kuperberg, “Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: challenges to syntax,” Brain Research, vol. 1146, no. 1, pp. 23–49, 2007.
[40]
J. S. Lawson, A. McGhie, and J. Chapman, “Perception of speech in schizophrenia,” The British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 110, pp. 375–380, 1964.
[41]
R. Levy and A. E. Maxwell, “The effect of verbal context on the recall of schizophrenics and other psychiatric patients,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 114, no. 508, pp. 311–316, 1968.
[42]
B. A. Maher, T. C. Manschreck, and M. E. Rucklos, “Contextual constraint and the recall of verbal material in schizophrenia: the effect of thought disorder,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 137, no. 1, pp. 69–73, 1980.
[43]
T. C. Manschreck, B. A. Maher, J. E. Rosenthal, and J. Berner, “Reduced primacy and related features in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 35–41, 1991.
[44]
T. Ditman and G. R. Kuperberg, “The time course of building discourse coherence in schizophrenia: an ERP investigation,” Psychophysiology, vol. 44, no. 6, pp. 991–1001, 2007.
[45]
T. Ditman, D. Goff, and G. R. Kuperberg, “Slow and steady: sustained effects of lexico-semantic associations can mediate referential impairments in schizophrenia,” Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 245–258, 2011.