Jumping to Conclusions Is Associated with Paranoia but Not General Suspiciousness: A Comparison of Two Versions of the Probabilistic Reasoning Paradigm
Theoretical models ascribe jumping to conclusions (JTCs) a prominent role in the pathogenesis of paranoia. While many earlier studies corroborated this account, some newer investigations have found no or only small associations of the JTC bias with paranoid symptoms. The present study examined whether these inconsistencies in part reflect methodological differences across studies. The study was built upon the psychometric high-risk paradigm. A total of 1899 subjects from the general population took part in an online survey and were administered the Paranoia Checklist as well as one of two different variants of the probabilistic reasoning task: one variant with a traditional instruction (a) and one novel variant that combines probability estimates with decision judgments (b). Factor analysis of the Paranoia Checklist yielded an unspecific suspiciousness factor and a psychotic paranoia factor. The latter was significantly associated with scores indicating hasty decision making. Subjects scoring two standard deviations above the mean of the Paranoia Checklist showed an abnormal data-gathering style relative to subjects with normal scores. Findings suggest that the so-called decision threshold parameter is more sensitive than the conventional JTC index. For future research the specific contents of paranoid beliefs deserve more consideration in the investigation of decision making in schizophrenia as JTC seems to be associated with core psychosis-prone features of paranoia only. 1. Introduction Research on neuropsychological dysfunctions in schizophrenia (e.g., memory and executive dysfunction) has been increasingly extended by studies on cognitive biases [1–3]. Cognitive biases represent preferences, subtle distortions, and styles of information processing rather than neural deficits or mere inaccuracy [1]. An emerging literature has elucidated that persons with delusions tend to jump to conclusions [3, 4], are over-confident in their incorrect decisions [5–9], and show attributional biases [10–12], for example, a preference for monocausal inferences [13], and a bias against disconfirmatory evidence [14–18]. Some of these biases have been found to correlate with positive symptoms (i.e., delusions and hallucinations), which according to many clinicians represent the core of the disorder. Cognitive training programs such as the Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) [19, 20], the Maudsley Review Training Program [21] or the Metacognitive Training for Psychosis (MCT) [22, 23] have begun to translate these insights into practice: patients learn to
References
[1]
S. Moritz, F. Vitzthum, S. Randjbar, R. Veckenstedt, and T. S. Woodward, “Detecting and defusing cognitive traps: metacognitive intervention in schizophrenia,” Current Opinion in Psychiatry, vol. 23, no. 6, pp. 561–569, 2010.
[2]
M. van der Gaag, “A neuropsychiatric model of biological and psychological processes in the remission of delusions and auditory hallucinations,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, pp. S113–122, 2006.
[3]
D. Freeman, “Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions,” Clinical Psychology Review, vol. 27, no. 4, pp. 425–457, 2007.
[4]
C. Fine, M. Gardner, J. Craigie, and I. Gold, “Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 46–77, 2007.
[5]
M. J. V. Peters, M. J. Cima, T. Smeets, M. De Vos, M. Jelicic, and H. Merckelbach, “Did I say that word or did you? Executive dysfunctions in schizophrenic patients affect memory efficiency, but not source attributions,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 12, no. 5, pp. 391–411, 2007.
[6]
S. Moritz, T. S. Woodward, L. Jelinek, and R. Klinge, “Memory and metamemory in schizophrenia: a liberal acceptance account of psychosis,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 38, no. 6, pp. 825–832, 2008.
[7]
S. Moritz, T. S. Woodward, and R. Rodriguez-Raecke, “Patients with schizophrenia do not produce more false memories than controls but are more confident in them,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 36, no. 5, pp. 659–667, 2006.
[8]
S. Moritz and T. S. Woodward, “The contribution of metamemory deficits to schizophrenia,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 115, no. 1, pp. 15–25, 2006.
[9]
?. Gaweda, S. Moritz, and A. Kokoszka, “Impaired discrimination between imagined and performed actions in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 195, no. 1-2, pp. 1–8, 2012.
[10]
T. M. Lincoln, S. Mehl, C. Exner, J. Lindenmeyer, and W. Rief, “Attributional style and persecutory delusions. Evidence for an event independent and state specific external-personal attribution bias for social situations,” Cognitive Therapy and Research, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 297–302, 2010.
[11]
R. P. Bentall, “Cognitive biases and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions,” in The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia, A. S. David and J. Cutting, Eds., pp. 337–360, Erlbaum, London, UK, 1994.
[12]
P. Kinderman and R. P. Bentall, “Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: internal, personal, and situational attributions for negative events,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 106, no. 2, pp. 341–345, 1997.
[13]
S. Randjbar, R. Veckenstedt, F. Vitzthum, B. Hottenrott, and S. Moritz, “Attributional biases in paranoid schizophrenia: further evidence for a decreased sense of self-causation in paranoia,” Psychosis, vol. 3, pp. 74–85, 2011.
[14]
T. S. Woodward, W. J. Speechley, S. Moritz, T. Lecomte, and E. Ngan, “The contribution of integrating disconfirmatory evidence and jumping to conclusions to delusions in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 35, pp. 249–249, 2009.
[15]
T. S. Woodward, L. Buchy, S. Moritz, and M. Liotti, “A bias against disconfirmatory evidence is associated with delusion proneness in a nonclinical sample,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 33, no. 4, pp. 1023–1028, 2007.
[16]
L. Buchy, T. S. Woodward, and M. Liotti, “A cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) is associated with schizotypy,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 90, no. 1–3, pp. 334–337, 2007.
[17]
S. Moritz and T. S. Woodward, “A generalized bias against disconfirmatory evidence in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 142, no. 2-3, pp. 157–165, 2006.
[18]
T. S. Woodward, S. Moritz, C. Cuttler, and J. C. Whitman, “The contribution of a cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) to delusions in schizophrenia,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 605–617, 2006.
[19]
D. R. Combs, S. D. Adams, D. L. Penn, D. Roberts, J. Tiegreen, and P. Stem, “Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 33, no. 2, article 585, 2007.
[20]
D. L. Roberts and D. L. Penn, “Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia: a preliminary study,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 166, no. 2-3, pp. 141–147, 2009.
[21]
H. Waller, D. Freeman, S. Jolley, G. Dunn, and P. Garety, “Targeting reasoning biases in delusions: a pilot study of the Maudsley Review Training Programme for individuals with persistent, high conviction delusions,” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, vol. 42, no. 3, pp. 414–421, 2011.
[22]
S. Moritz, A. Kerstan, R. Veckenstedt et al., “Further evidence for the efficacy of a metacognitive group training in schizophrenia,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 49, no. 3, pp. 151–157, 2011.
[23]
S. Moritz, R. Veckenstedt, S. Randjbar, F. Vitzthum, and T. S. Woodward, “Antipsychotic treatment beyond antipsychotics: metacognitive intervention for schizophrenia patients improves delusional symptoms,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 41, no. 09, pp. 1823–1832, 2011.
[24]
P. A. Garety, D. R. Hemsley, and S. Wessely, “Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Biases in performance on a probabilistic inference task,” Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, vol. 179, no. 4, pp. 194–201, 1991.
[25]
M. Ziegler, W. Rief, S. M. Werner, S. Mehl, and T. M. Lincoln, “Hasty decision-making in a variety of tasks: does it contribute to the development of delusions?” Psychology and Psychotherapy, vol. 81, no. 3, pp. 237–245, 2008.
[26]
P. A. Garety, D. Freeman, S. Jolley et al., “Reasoning, emotions, and delusional conviction in psychosis,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 114, no. 3, pp. 373–384, 2005.
[27]
D. M. Warman, P. H. Lysaker, J. M. Martin, L. Davis, and S. L. Haudenschield, “Jumping to conclusions and the continuum of delusional beliefs,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 45, no. 6, pp. 1255–1269, 2007.
[28]
R. Langdon, P. B. Ward, and M. Coltheart, “Reasoning anomalies associated with delusions in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 321–330, 2010.
[29]
T. M. Lincoln, M. Ziegler, S. Mehl, and W. Rief, “The jumping to conclusions bias in delusions: specificity and changeability,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 119, no. 1, pp. 40–49, 2010.
[30]
T. S. Woodward, M. Munz, C. LeClerc, and T. Lecomte, “Change in delusions is associated with change in ‘jumping to conclusions’,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 170, no. 2-3, pp. 124–127, 2009.
[31]
S. H. So, D. Freeman, G. Dunn et al., “Jumping to conclusions, a lack of belief flexibility and delusional conviction in psychosis: a longitudinal investigation of the structure, frequency, and relatedness of reasoning biases,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 121, no. 1, pp. 129–139, 2012.
[32]
M. Menon, R. Mizrahi, and S. Kapur, “‘Jumping to conclusions’ and delusions in psychosis: relationship and response to treatment,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 98, no. 1–3, pp. 225–231, 2008.
[33]
T. S. Woodward, R. Mizrahi, M. Menon, and B. K. Christensen, “Correspondences between theory of mind, jumping to conclusions, neuropsychological measures and the symptoms of schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 170, no. 2-3, pp. 119–123, 2009.
[34]
R. Dudley, D. Shaftoe, K. Cavanagh et al., “‘Jumping to conclusions’ in first-episode psychosis,” Early Intervention in Psychiatry, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 50–56, 2011.
[35]
D. Freeman, K. Pugh, N. Vorontsova, A. Antley, and M. Slater, “Testing the continuum of delusional beliefs: an experimental study using virtual reality,” Journal of Abnormal Psychology, vol. 119, no. 1, pp. 83–92, 2010.
[36]
F. Van Dael, D. Versmissen, I. Janssen, I. Myin-Germeys, J. Van Os, and L. Krabbendam, “Data gathering: biased in psychosis?” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 341–351, 2006.
[37]
S. Moritz and T. S. Woodward, “Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non-delusional schizophrenic patients,” British Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 44, no. 2, pp. 193–207, 2005.
[38]
H. Startup, D. Freeman, and P. A. Garety, “Jumping to conclusions and persecutory delusions,” European Psychiatry, vol. 23, no. 6, pp. 457–459, 2008.
[39]
P. A. Garety, M. Gittins, S. Jolley, et al., “Differences in cognitive and emotional processes between persecutory and grandiose delusions,” Schizophrenia Bulletin. In press.
[40]
C. Andreou, D. Roesch-Ely, R. Veckenstedt, F. Vitzthum, and S. Moritz, “Cognitive biases as predictors of symptomatic outcome in schizophrenia,” in Proceedings of the 20th European Conference of Psychiatry, Prague, Czech Republic, 2012.
[41]
C. Andreou, D. Roesch-Ely, R. Veckenstedt, et al., “Predictors of symptomatic remission after an exacerbation of schizophrenia: the significance of symptoms, neuropsychological performance and cognitive biases,” submitted.
[42]
M. Wagner, M. Landsberg, S. Mehl, et al., “Cognitive biases and cognitive deficits in patients with positive symptoms,” in European Conference on Schizophrenia Research, Berlin, Germany, 2011.
[43]
S. Moritz, R. Veckenstedt, B. Hottenrott, T. S. Woodward, S. Randjbar, and T. M. Lincoln, “Different sides of the same coin? Intercorrelations of cognitive biases in schizophrenia,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 406–421, 2010.
[44]
S. Moritz, R. Veckenstedt, S. Randjbar et al., “Decision making under uncertainty and mood induction: further evidence for liberal acceptance in schizophrenia,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 39, no. 11, pp. 1821–1829, 2009.
[45]
S. Moritz, T. S. Woodward, and M. Lambert, “Under what circumtances do patients with schizophrenia jump to conclusions? A liberal acceptance account,” British Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 46, no. 2, pp. 127–137, 2007.
[46]
R. Miller, “Schizophrenic psychology, associative learning and the role of forebrain dopamine,” Medical Hypotheses, vol. 2, no. 5, pp. 203–211, 1976.
[47]
S. Moritz, T. S. Woodward, and D. Hausmann, “Incautious reasoning as a pathogenetic factor for the development of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 327–331, 2006.
[48]
R. Veckenstedt, S. Randjbar, F. Vitzthum, B. Hottenrott, T. S. Woodward, and S. Moritz, “Incorrigibility, jumping to conclusions, and decision threshold in schizophrenia,” Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, vol. 16, no. 2, pp. 174–192, 2011.
[49]
F. Scheu, S. Moritz, J. Aghotor, et al., “Liberal acceptance and jumping to conclusions in schizophrenia,” in International Schizophrenia Symposium Bern (ISSB '10), Bern, Switzerland, 2010.
[50]
S. Moritz and T. S. Woodward, “Metacognitive control over false memories: a key determinant of delusional thinking,” Current Psychiatry Reports, vol. 8, no. 3, pp. 184–190, 2006.
[51]
S. R. Kay, L. A. Opler, and J. P. Lindenmayer, “The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS): rationale and standardisation,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 155, supplement 7, pp. 59–65, 1989.
[52]
E. R. Peters, S. A. Joseph, and P. A. Garety, “Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory),” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 553–576, 1999.
[53]
D. Freeman, P. A. Garety, P. E. Bebbington et al., “Psychological investigation of the structure of paranoia in a non-clinical population,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 186, pp. 427–435, 2005.
[54]
G. Haddock, J. McCarron, N. Tarrier, and E. B. Faragher, “Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS),” Psychological Medicine, vol. 29, no. 4, pp. 879–889, 1999.
[55]
T. M. Lincoln, S. Salzmann, M. Ziegler, and S. Westermann, “When does jumping-to-conclusions reach its peak? the interaction of vulnerability and situation-characteristics in social reasoning,” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, vol. 42, no. 2, pp. 185–191, 2011.
[56]
L. J. Chapman and J. P. Chapman, “Psychosis proneness,” in Controversies in Schizophrenia: Changes and Constancies, M. Alpert, Ed., pp. 157–174, Guilford Press, New York, NY, USA, 1985.
[57]
M. F. Lenzenweger and L. Korfine, “Perceptual aberrations, schizotypy, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 20, no. 2, pp. 345–357, 1994.
[58]
T. A. Judge, R. Ilies, and B. A. Scott, “Work-family conflict and emotions: effects at work and at home,” Personnel Psychology, vol. 59, no. 4, pp. 779–814, 2006.
[59]
A. S. G?ritz, “Using online panels in psychological research,” in The Oxford Handbook of Internet Psychology, A. N. Joinson, K. Y. A. McKenna, T. Postmes, and U.-D. Reips, Eds., pp. 473–485, Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 2007.
[60]
R. F. Piccolo and J. A. Colquitt, “Transformational leadership and job behaviors: the mediating role of core job characteristics,” Academy of Management Journal, vol. 49, no. 2, pp. 327–340, 2006.
[61]
EFS Survey Unipark Program [computer program]. Version 8.1. K?ln, Germany, 2011.
[62]
T. M. Lincoln, N. Peter, M. Sch?fer, and S. Moritz, “Research Letter: from stress to paranoia: an experimental investigation of the moderating and mediating role of reasoning biases,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 169–171, 2010.
[63]
T. M. Lincoln, M. Ziegler, E. Lüllmann, M. J. Müller, and W. Rief, “Can delusions be self-assessed? Concordance between self- and observer-rated delusions in schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 178, no. 2, pp. 249–254, 2010.
[64]
S. Westermann, M.-L. Kesting, and T. M. Lincoln, “Being deluded after being excluded?—how emotion regulation deficits in paranoia-prone individuals impact on state paranoia during experimentally induced social stress,” Behavior Therapy, vol. 43, no. 2, pp. 329–340.
[65]
S. Westermann and T. M. Lincoln, “Using signal detection theory to test the impact of negative emotion on sub-clinical paranoia,” Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, vol. 41, no. 2, pp. 96–101, 2010.
[66]
J. Lehrer, “The truth wears off—is there something wrong with the scientific method?” Annals of Science, The New Yorker—Weekly Magazine, 2010.
[67]
C. E. L. Green, D. Freeman, E. Kuipers et al., “Measuring ideas of persecution and social reference: the Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scales (GPTS),” Psychological Medicine, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 101–111, 2008.
[68]
P. Jacobsen, D. Freeman, and P. Salkovskis, “Reasoning bias and belief conviction in obsessive-compulsive disorder and delusions: jumping to conclusions across disorders?” British Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 51, no. 1, pp. 84–99, 2012.
[69]
S. Moritz, N. Van Quaquebeke, M. Hauschildt, L. Jelinek, and S. G?nner, “Good news for allegedly bad studies. Deception in psychopathological studies is less easy than it seems,” Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, vol. 39, pp. 331–335, 2012.
[70]
S. Moritz, R. Veckenstedt, S. Randjbar et al., “Decision making under uncertainty and mood induction: further evidence for liberal acceptance in schizophrenia,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 39, no. 11, pp. 1821–1829, 2009.
[71]
S. Westermann, S. Salzmann, X. Fuchs, and T. M. Lincoln, “Introducing a social beads task,” Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. In press.
[72]
W. J. Speechley, J. C. Whitman, and T. S. Woodward, “The contribution of hypersalience to the “jumping to conclusions” bias associated with delusions in schizophrenia,” Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 7–17, 2010.