Social cognition is a domain of cognitive function that includes the ability to understand and manage social interactions. Emotional intelligence (EI) has been identified as a component of social cognition and is defined as the ability to identify, use, understand, and manage emotions. Neurocognitive impairments are known to be associated with poorer social function in people with schizophrenia, but less is known about the relationships between EI, neurocognition, and social function. The current study assessed EI using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) in 20 people with schizophrenia and 20 controls. The schizophrenia group had significantly lower scores on all measures of EI and demonstrated poorer neurocognition and social functioning than controls. The difference between schizophrenia and control groups was greatest for the Understanding Emotions Branch of the MSCEIT. The neurocognition score and total EI score accounted for 18.3% of the variance in social function in the control group and 9.1% of the variance in social function in the schizophrenia group. Our results suggest that a total EI score is not a useful predictor of overall social function and it may be more clinically useful to develop an individual profile of social cognitive abilities, including EI, to form a remediation program. 1. Introduction Social cognition refers to aspects of cognition that are critical for social functioning and interpersonal relationships, such as accurate perception of the emotions of others, the ability to recognize important social cues, theory of mind (TOM), and the management of emotions in social situations [1]. Various components of social cognition have been found to be impaired in people with schizophrenia [2, 3]. Impaired social cognition may also be a risk factor for psychosis—Kim et al. [4] reported that poor performance on social cognition tasks was associated with higher rates of transition to psychosis in young people who were at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Emotional intelligence (EI) is an important component of social cognition [5, 6]. EI is defined as the ability to process, understand, and manage emotions. EI is most commonly assessed using the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test, Version 2.0 (MSCEIT) [7, 8]. Mayer and Salovey [6] identified four components (termed Branches) of EI. The four Branches are Identifying Emotions (the perception of emotion in the self and others); Facilitating or Using Emotions (the capacity to use and generate emotions, and to use emotions in cognitive tasks such as
References
[1]
M. F. Green, B. Olivier, J. N. Crawley, D. L. Penn, and S. Silverstein, “Social cognition in schizophrenia: recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 31, no. 4, pp. 882–887, 2005.
[2]
E. Bora, M. Yucel, and C. Pantelis, “Theory of mind impairment in schizophrenia: meta-analysis,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 109, no. 1–3, pp. 1–9, 2009.
[3]
P. Stratta, M. Bustini, E. Daneluzzo, I. Riccardi, M. D'Arcangelo, and A. Rossi, “Deconstructing theory of mind in Schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 190, no. 1, pp. 32–36, 2011.
[4]
H. S. Kim, N. Y. Shin, J. H. Jang et al., “Social cognition and neurocognition as predictors of conversion to psychosis in individuals at ultra-high risk,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 130, no. 1–3, pp. 170–175, 2011.
[5]
G. Matthews, M. Zeidner, and R. D. Roberts, “Emotional intelligence: consensus, controversies, and questions,” in The Science of Emotional Intelligence, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
[6]
J. D. Mayer and P. Salovey, “What is emotional intelligence?” in Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Educational Implications, Basic Books, New York, NY, USA, 1997.
[7]
J. D. Mayer, P. Salovey, and D. R. Caruso, Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) User's Manual, MHS, Toronto, Canada, 2002.
[8]
J. D. Mayer, P. Salovey, D. R. Caruso, and G. Sitarenios, “Measuring Emotional Intelligence with the MSCEIT V2.0,” Emotion, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 97–105, 2003.
[9]
P. N. Lopes, P. Salovey, S. C?té, and M. Beers, “Emotion regulation abilities and the quality of social interaction,” Emotion, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 113–118, 2005.
[10]
M. A. Brackett and J. D. Mayer, “Convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, vol. 29, no. 9, pp. 1147–1158, 2003.
[11]
D. R. Trinidad and C. A. Johnson, “The association between emotional intelligence and early adolescent tobacco and alcohol use,” Personality and Individual Differences, vol. 32, no. 1, pp. 95–105, 2002.
[12]
N. R. Burns, V. A. Bastian, and T. Nettelbeck, “Emotional intelligence: more than personality and cognitive ability?” in The Science of Emotional Intelligence, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2007.
[13]
K. H. Nuechterlein, M. F. Green, R. S. Kern et al., “The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity,” American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 165, no. 2, pp. 203–213, 2008.
[14]
S. M. August, J. N. Kiwanuka, R. P. McMahon, and J. M. Gold, “The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB): clinical and cognitive correlates,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 76–82, 2012.
[15]
S. M. Eack, C. G. Greeno, M. F. Pogue-Geile, C. E. Newhill, G. E. Hogarty, and M. S. Keshavan, “Assessing social-cognitive deficits in schizophrenia with the mayer-salovey-caruso emotional intelligence test,” Schizophrenia Bulletin, vol. 36, no. 2, pp. 370–380, 2010.
[16]
K. S. Kee, W. P. Horan, P. Salovey et al., “Emotional intelligence in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 107, no. 1, pp. 61–68, 2009.
[17]
J. van Os and S. Kapur, “Schizophrenia,” The Lancet, vol. 374, no. 9690, pp. 635–645, 2009.
[18]
S. Shamsi, A. Lau, T. Lencz et al., “Cognitive and symptomatic predictors of functional disability in schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 126, no. 1–3, pp. 257–264, 2011.
[19]
M. Brüne, M. Abdel-Hamid, C. Lehmk?mper, and C. Sonntag, “Mental state attribution, neurocognitive functioning, and psychopathology: what predicts poor social competence in schizophrenia best?” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 92, no. 1–3, pp. 151–159, 2007.
[20]
A. K. J. Fett, W. Viechtbauer, M. D. G. Dominguez, D. L. Penn, J. van Os, and L. Krabbendam, “The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 35, no. 3, pp. 573–588, 2011.
[21]
S. M. Eack, M. F. Pogue-Geile, D. P. Greenwald, S. S. Hogarty, and M. S. Keshavan, “Mechanisms of functional improvement in a 2-year trial of cognitive enhancement therapy for early schizophrenia,” Psychological Medicine, vol. 41, no. 6, pp. 1253–1261, 2011.
[22]
American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC, USA, 4th edition, 2000.
[23]
D. V. Sheehan, Y. Lecrubier, K. H. Sheehan et al., “The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10,” Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, vol. 59, no. 20, pp. 22–33, 1998.
[24]
P. Salovey and D. A. Pizarro, “The value of emotional intelligence,” in Models of Intelligence, American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, USA, 2004.
[25]
J. D. Mayer, P. Salovey, and D. R. Caruso, “Emotional intelligence: theory, findings, and implications,” Psychological Inquiry, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 197–215, 2004.
[26]
H. Nelson and J. Willson, National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual, Nelson Publishing, Windsor, UK, 2nd edition, 1991.
[27]
R. S. E. Keefe, P. D. Harvey, T. E. Goldberg et al., “Norms and standardization of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS),” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 102, no. 1–3, pp. 108–115, 2008.
[28]
R. S. E. Keefe, T. E. Goldberg, P. D. Harvey, J. M. Gold, M. P. Poe, and L. Coughenour, “The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 68, no. 2-3, pp. 283–297, 2004.
[29]
M. Birchwood, J. Smith, R. Cochrane, S. Wetton, and S. Copestake, “The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients,” British Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 157, pp. 853–859, 1990.
[30]
G. G. Pijnenborg, J. M. Spikman, B. F. Jeronimus, and A. Aleman, “Insight in schizophrenia: associations with empathy,” European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. In press.
[31]
S. K. Chan, K. K. Chan, M. M. Lam, et al., “Clinical and cognitive correlates of insight in first-episode schizophrenia,” Schizophrenia Research, vol. 135, no. 1–3, pp. 40–45, 2012.
[32]
M. Erickson, N. Jaafari, and P. Lysaker, “Insight and negative symptoms as predictors of functioning in a work setting in patients with schizophrenia,” Psychiatry Research, vol. 189, no. 2, pp. 161–165, 2011.