We examined whether people with Parkinson disease (PD) have difficulty negotiating a gait obstruction in threatening (gait path and obstacle raised above floor) and nonthreatening (gait path and obstacle at floor level) contexts. Ten PD patients were tested in both Meds OFF and Meds ON states, along with 10 age-matched controls. Participants completed 18 gait trials, walking 4.7?m at a self-selected speed while attempting to cross an obstacle 0.15?m in height placed near the centre point of the walkway. Kinematic and kinetic parameters were measured, and obstacle contact errors were tallied. Results indicated that PD patients made more obstacle contacts than control participants in the threatening context. Successful crossings by PD patients in the threatening condition also exhibited kinematic differences, with Meds OFF PD patients making shorter crossing steps, with decreased initiation and crossing velocities. The findings from this study lend support to the theory that PD patients rely on directed attention to initiate and control movement, while providing indication that the motor improvements provided by current PD pharmacotherapy may be limited by contextual interference. These movement patterns may be placing PD patients at risk of obstacle contact and falling. 1. Introduction Epidemiological investigation indicates that Parkinson disease (PD) patients experience more falls than either age-matched healthy controls or individuals with other neuropathologies, including spinal disorders, epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, stroke, and motor neuron disease [1]. For patients with PD, fall occurrences and increased fear of falling are frequent in situations with complex or threatening context [2], with contact with an obstacle presenting a major cause of falls among PD [1, 3]. Task demands, such as the inherent characteristics of the obstacle to be crossed as well as constraints imposed by the general environment surrounding the obstacle and task, contribute to context [4] and exacerbate motor disturbances amongst PD patients [5]. Previous studies have shown that neurotypical adults adopt conservative strategies for standing [6, 7], walking [8], and obstacle crossing [9] when behaving in a context that threatens increased physical consequences as a result of a fall. In contrast, PD patients have exhibited increased postural instability [10] and gait disturbance [11] when concurrently challenged with a cognitive or motor demand. It is probable that threatening context may exacerbate any obstacle negotiation deficits that exist for PD patients. While PD
References
[1]
H. Stolze, S. Klebe, C. Zechlin, C. Baecker, L. Friege, and G. Deuschl, “Falls in frequent neurological diseases: prevalence, risk factors and aetiology,” Journal of Neurology, vol. 251, no. 1, pp. 79–84, 2004.
[2]
P. Gray and K. Hildebrand, “Fall risk factors in Parkinson's disease,” The Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 222–228, 2000.
[3]
B. R. Bloem, J. M. Hausdorff, J. E. Visser, and N. Giladi, “Falls and freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease: a review of two interconnected, episodic phenomena,” Movement Disorders, vol. 19, no. 8, pp. 871–884, 2004.
[4]
W. Dunn, C. Brown, and A. McGuigan, “The ecology of human performance: a framework for considering the effect of context,” The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, vol. 48, no. 7, pp. 595–607, 1994.
[5]
A. Nieuwboer, R. Dom, W. De Weerdt, K. Desloovere, S. Fieuws, and E. Broens-Kaucsik, “Abnormalities of the spatiotemporal characteristics of gait at the onset of freezing in Parkinson's disease,” Movement Disorders, vol. 16, no. 6, pp. 1066–1075, 2001.
[6]
A. L. Adkin, J. S. Frank, M. G. Carpenter, and G. W. Peysar, “Postural control is scaled to level of postural threat,” Gait & Posture, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 87–93, 2000.
[7]
L. A. Brown, M. A. Polych, and J. B. Doan, “The effect of anxiety on the regulation of upright standing among younger and older adults,” Gait & Posture, vol. 24, no. 4, pp. 397–405, 2006.
[8]
L. A. Brown, W. H. Gage, M. A. Polych, R. J. Sleik, and T. R. Winder, “Central set influences on gait: age-dependent effects of postural threat,” Experimental Brain Research, vol. 145, no. 3, pp. 286–296, 2002.
[9]
N. C. McKenzie and L. A. Brown, “Obstacle negotiation kinematics: age-dependent effects of postural threat,” Gait & Posture, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 226–234, 2004.
[10]
M. E. Morris, R. Iansek, F. Smithson, and F. Huxham, “Postural instability in Parkinson's disease: a comparison with and without a concurrent task,” Gait & Posture, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 205–216, 2000.
[11]
L. Rochester, V. Hetherington, D. Jones et al., “Attending to the task: interference effects of functional tasks on walking in Parkinson's disease and the roles of cognition, depression, fatigue, and balance,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 85, no. 10, pp. 1578–1585, 2004.
[12]
M. Vokaer, N. Abou Azar, and D. Zegers De Beyl, “Effects of levodopa on upper limb mobility and gait in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, vol. 74, no. 9, pp. 1304–1307, 2003.
[13]
O. Blin, A. M. Ferrandez, J. Pailhous, and G. Serratrice, “Dopa-sensitive and dopa resistant gait parameters in Parkinson's disease,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 103, no. 1, pp. 51–54, 1991.
[14]
K. G. Melvin, J. B. Doan, S. M. Pellis, L. A. Brown, I. Q. Whishaw, and O. Suchowersky, “Pallidal deep brain stimulation and L-dopa do not improve qualitative aspects of skilled reaching in Parkinson's disease,” Behavioural Brain Research, vol. 160, no. 1, pp. 188–194, 2005.
[15]
J. B. Doan, I. Q. Whishaw, S. M. Pellis, O. Suchowersky, and L. A. Brown, “Motor deficits in Parkinsonian reaching: dopa-sensitivity influenced by real-world task constraint,” Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 45–59, 2006.
[16]
J. D. Schaafsma, N. Giladi, Y. Balash, A. L. Bartels, T. Gurevich, and J. M. Hausdorff, “Gait dynamics in Parkinson's disease: relationship to Parkinsonian features, falls and response to levodopa,” Journal of the Neurological Sciences, vol. 212, no. 1-2, pp. 47–53, 2003.
[17]
A. Ashburn, E. Stack, R. M. Pickering, and C. D. Ward, “A community-dwelling sample of people with Parkinson's disesase: characteristics of fallers and non-fallers,” Age and Ageing, vol. 30, no. 1, pp. 47–52, 2001.
[18]
H.-C. Chen, J. A. Ashton-Miller, N. B. Alexander, and A. B. Schultz, “Effects of age and available response time on ability to step over an obstacle,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 49, no. 5, pp. M227–M233, 1994.
[19]
D. A. Winter, Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA, 3rd edition.
[20]
S. E. Halliday, D. A. Winter, J. S. Frank, A. E. Patla, and F. Prince, “The initiation of gait in young, elderly, and Parkinson's disease subjects,” Gait & Posture, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 8–14, 1998.
[21]
M. E. Morris, R. Iansek, J. McGinley, T. Matyas, and F. Huxham, “Three-dimensional gait biomechanics in Parkinson's disease: evidence for a centrally mediated amplitude regulation disorder,” Movement Disorders, vol. 20, no. 1, pp. 40–50, 2005.
[22]
M. D. Bishop, D. Brunt, C. Kukulka, M. D. Tillman, and N. Pathare, “Braking impulse and muscle activation during unplanned gait termination in human subjects with parkinsonism,” Neuroscience Letters, vol. 348, no. 2, pp. 89–92, 2003.
[23]
V. Weerdesteyn, A. M. Schillings, G. P. van Galen, and J. Duysens, “Distraction affects the performance of obstacle avoidance during walking,” Journal of Motor Behavior, vol. 35, no. 1, pp. 53–63, 2003.
[24]
H.-D. Kim, “Effects of a dual-task on crossing an obstacle versus initiating gait,” Journal of Physical Therapy Science, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 249–253, 2008.
[25]
E. Koster, G. Crombez, B. Verschuere, and J. De Houwer, “Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage,” Behaviour Research and Therapy, vol. 42, no. 10, pp. 1183–1192, 2004.
[26]
R. Camicioli, B. S. Oken, G. Sexton, J. A. Kaye, and J. G. Nutt, “Verbal fluency task affects gait in Parkinson's disease with motor freezing,” Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, vol. 11, no. 4, pp. 181–185, 1998.
[27]
G. Dirnberger, C. D. Frith, and M. Jahanshahi, “Executive dysfunction in Parkinson's disease is associated with altered pallidal-frontal processing,” NeuroImage, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 588–599, 2005.
[28]
K. Walsh and G. Bennett, “Parkinson's disease and anxiety,” Postgraduate Medical Journal, vol. 77, no. 904, pp. 89–93, 2001.
[29]
A. L. Adkin, J. S. Frank, and M. S. Jog, “Fear of falling and postural control in Parkinson's disease,” Movement Disorders, vol. 18, no. 5, pp. 496–502, 2003.
[30]
M. E. Morris, R. Iansek, T. A. Matyas, and J. J. Summers, “The pathogenesis of gait hypokinesia in Parkinson's disease,” Brain, vol. 117, no. 5, pp. 1169–1181, 1994.