The purpose of this study was to investigate the effect of stereoscopic display alignment errors on visual fatigue and prefrontal cortical tissue hemodynamic responses. We collected hemodynamic data and perceptual ratings of visual fatigue while participants performed visual display tasks on 8?ft 6?ft NEC LT silver screen with NEC LT 245 DLP projectors. There was statistical significant difference between subjective measures of visual fatigue before air traffic control task (BATC) and after air traffic control task (ATC 3), ( ). Statistical significance was observed between left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex oxygenated hemoglobin (l DLPFC-HbO2), left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex deoxygenated hemoglobin (l DLPFC-Hbb), and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex deoxygenated hemoglobin (r DLPFC-Hbb) on stereoscopic alignment errors ( ). Thus, cortical tissue oxygenation requirement in the left hemisphere indicates that the effect of visual fatigue is more pronounced in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. 1. Introduction The visual system is a well-developed sensory organ that involves effortless complex interaction between the visual organ and the brain. It involves physiological and a series of hierarchical neural processes from the photoreceptors, retinal ganglion cells, the lateral geniculate nuclei (LGN) of the thalamus, and the visual cortices [1]. The American Optometric Association [2] identified that prolonged use of the visual system can result in inefficient visual processing functions generally called visual fatigue. In line with that, the American Association of Ophthalmologist (AAO) diagnosed visual fatigue as an ophthalmological disease called asthenopia with the medical diagnostic code of ICD-9, 368.13 with the subclassifications shown in Table 1 [3, 4]. The last two digits of the medical code indicate the seriousness of the ophthalmological disease. The larger the last two digits, the more severe the symptoms of the disease. Table 1: Ophthalmological classification of visual fatigue (asthenopia) with their respective symptoms. Visual fatigue commonly arises when people view stereoscopic motion images [5]. In a stereoscopic image, the viewer watches two images corresponding to the right and left eyes with convergence eye movement. Based on individual characteristics and instrumentation issues, conflicts can occur between convergence or divergence eye movements and the accommodation function thus causing visual fatigue [6]. One of the major problems with stereoscopic displays is that a stereoscopic display produces a mismatch between
References
[1]
G. C. DeAngelis, “Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 80–90, 2000.
[2]
American Optometric Association, Guide to the Clinical Aspects of Computer Vision Syndrome, American Optometric Association, St. Louis, Mo, USA, 1999.
[3]
National Center for Health Statistics, International Classification of Diseases 9th Revision Clinical Modification: Physician ICD-9-CM, Division of Data Services, Hyattsville, Md, USA, 2003.
[4]
J. E. Sheedy, J. Hayes, and J. Engle, “Is all asthenopia the same?” Optometry and Vision Science, vol. 80, no. 11, pp. 732–739, 2003.
[5]
K. Ukai and P. A. Howarth, “Visual fatigue caused by viewing stereoscopic motion images: background, theories, and observations,” Displays, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 106–116, 2008.
[6]
S. Pastoor and M. W?pking, “3-D displays: a review of current technologies,” Displays, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 100–110, 1997.
[7]
E. W. Jin, System Considerations for Optimizing Stereo Image Quality, Aptina Imaging Corporation, 2011.
[8]
T. Inoue and H. Ohzu, “Accommodative responses to stereoscopic three-dimensional display,” Applied Optics, vol. 36, no. 19, pp. 4509–4515, 1997.
[9]
Y. Okada, K. Ukai, J. S. Wolffsohn, B. Gilmartin, A. Iijima, and T. Bando, “Target spatial frequency determines the response to conflicting defocus- and convergence-driven accommodative stimuli,” Vision Research, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 475–484, 2006.
[10]
T. Fukushima, M. Torii, K. Ukai, J. S. Wolffsohn, and B. Gilmartin, “The relationship between CA/C ratio and individual differences in dynamic accommodative responses while viewing stereoscopic images,” Journal of Vision, vol. 9, no. 13, pp. 1–13, 2009.
[11]
J. Kuze and K. Ukai, “Subjective evaluation of visual fatigue caused by motion images,” Displays, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 159–166, 2008.
[12]
H. C. Self, “Optical tolerances for alignment and image differences for binocular helmet-mounted displays,” Tech. Rep. AAMRL-TR-86-019, 1986.
[13]
F. L. Kooi and A. Toet, “Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays,” Displays, vol. 25, no. 2-3, pp. 99–108, 2004.
[14]
R. S. Kennedy, N. E. Lane, K. S. Berbaum, and M. G. Lilienthal, “Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness,” International Journal of Aviation Psychology, vol. 3, pp. 203–220, 1993.
[15]
K. Izzetoglu, S. Bunce, B. Onaral, K. Pourrezaei, and B. Chance, “Functional optical brain imaging using near-infrared during cognitive tasks,” International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 211–227, 2004.
[16]
R. D. Rolfe, “The role of probiotic cultures in the control of gastrointestinal health,” The Journal of Nutrition, vol. 130, no. 2, pp. 396S–402S, 2000.
[17]
G. Strangman, D. A. Boas, and J. P. Sutton, “Non-invasive neuroimaging using near-infrared light,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 52, no. 7, pp. 679–693, 2002.
[18]
B. Chance, S. Nioka, and Q. Luo, “Optical system and method for non-invasive imaging of biological tissue,” Google patents, 1998.
[19]
H. D. Wiyor, A Model of Dynamic Situation Awareness, Industrial and Systems Engineering, North Carolina Agriculture and Technical State University, Greensboro, NC, USA, 2009.
[20]
J. Travis and J. Kring, LabVIEW for Everyone, Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2002.
[21]
V. D. Hopkins, Human Factors in Air Traffic Control, Taylor & Francis, London, UK, 1995.
[22]
M. Millodot and I. Newton, “VEP measurement of the amplitude of accommodation,” British Journal of Ophthalmology, vol. 65, no. 4, pp. 294–298, 1981.
[23]
H. A. Stein and R. M. Stein, The Ophthalmic Assistant: A Guide for Ophthalmic Medical Personnel, vol. 6, Mosby, St. Louis, Mo, USA, 1994.
[24]
National Eye Institute, Why Fund the National Eye Institute? Eye Diseases Health and Research Information, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Alliance for Eye and Vision Research (NAEVR), Bethesda, Md, USA, 2002.
[25]
M. S. Nolan, Fundamentals of Air Traffic Control, Wadsworth Publishing Company, Belmont, Calif, USA, 1994.
[26]
A. C. Merzagora, M. T. Schultheis, B. Onaral, and M. Izzetoglu, “Functional near-infrared spectroscopy-based assessment of attention impairments after traumatic brain injury,” Journal of Innovative Optical Health Sciences, vol. 4, no. 3, pp. 251–260, 2011.
[27]
R. Cabeza and L. Nyberg, “Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 12, no. 1, pp. 1–47, 2000.
[28]
fNIR Devices LLC, FNIR Imager & COBI Studio Manual, fNIR Devices LLC, Potomac, Md, USA, 2010.
[29]
T. Pang-Ning, M. Steinbach, and V. Kumar, Introduction to Data Mining, Pearson Addison Wesley, Boston, Mass, USA, 2006.
[30]
SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC ,USA, 2008.
[31]
L. J. Cronbach, “Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests,” Psychometrika, vol. 16, no. 3, pp. 297–334, 1951.
[32]
L. Hatcher, A Step-by-Step Approach to Using the SAS (R) System for Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA, 1994.
[33]
J. Nunnaly, Psychometric Theory, McGraw-Hill, New York, NY, USA, 1978.
[34]
J. R. A. Santos, L. Lippke, and P. Pope, “PROC FACTOR: a tool for extracting hidden gems from a mountain of variables,” in Proceedings of the 23rd Annual SAS Users Group International Conference, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA, 1998.
[35]
R. B. Buxton, K. Uluda?, D. J. Dubowitz, and T. T. Liu, “Modeling the hemodynamic response to brain activation,” NeuroImage, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. S220–S233, 2004.
[36]
A. Hansen, “Disturbed ion gradients in brain anoxia,” Physiology, vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 54–57, 1987.
[37]
O. K. Ames III, “Concerns about the lack of professionalism: root causes rather than symptoms must be addressed,” The American Journal of Trial Advocacy, vol. 28, pp. 531–566, 2004.
[38]
P. Svensson, “A HEADACHE in 3-D, companies push technology despite illnesses,” in News & Record, p. B5, Greensboro, NC, USA, 2011.
[39]
R. M. R. Martínez, R. de la Escápita, J. de la Riva, and A. C. F. B. Rodriguez, Categorization of Fators Causing Athenopia in Reserach Center Professors at the ITCJ by Reading with VDT: A Shared Experience, Chih, Ciudad Juárez, Mexico, 2009.