全部 标题 作者
关键词 摘要

OALib Journal期刊
ISSN: 2333-9721
费用:99美元

查看量下载量

相关文章

更多...

Falls Risk and Simulated Driving Performance in Older Adults

DOI: 10.1155/2013/356948

Full-Text   Cite this paper   Add to My Lib

Abstract:

Declines in executive function and dual-task performance have been related to falls in older adults, and recent research suggests that older adults at risk for falls also show impairments on real-world tasks, such as crossing a street. The present study examined whether falls risk was associated with driving performance in a high-fidelity simulator. Participants were classified as high or low falls risk using the Physiological Profile Assessment and completed a number of challenging simulated driving assessments in which they responded quickly to unexpected events. High falls risk drivers had slower response times (~2.1 seconds) to unexpected events compared to low falls risk drivers (~1.7 seconds). Furthermore, when asked to perform a concurrent cognitive task while driving, high falls risk drivers showed greater costs to secondary task performance than did low falls risk drivers, and low falls risk older adults also outperformed high falls risk older adults on a computer-based measure of dual-task performance. Our results suggest that attentional differences between high and low falls risk older adults extend to simulated driving performance. 1. Introduction Per mile driven, adults over age 65 are more likely to be involved in motor vehicle collisions than are younger experienced drivers [1], and declines in attention are related to older driver impairment [2, 3]. Attention is also critical to balance and gait, especially for older adults. Walkers must monitor for changes in environment and plan their next step. When a walking or balance task is combined with a cognitively challenging secondary task (e.g., memorizing a list of words), performance decrements are found for both tasks relative to performing each task separately [4]. These dual-task costs suggest that walking competes for shared attentional resources, as predicted by resource models of attention [5–9]. Older adults often have increased difficulty when multitasking, including paradigms that involve balancing or walking [10–12]. For example, older adults show larger dual-task costs on walking while memorizing task than do younger adults [13–16]. Such declines in multitasking ability are theorized to result in an increased risk for falls among older adults. Approximately 30% of community-dwelling older adults experience one or more falls annually [17, 18]. Age-related declines in the ability to multitask are related to an increase in falls risk. For example, performance on a counting while walking task predicts falls in older adults ([19]; see also [20–22]). Similarly, older adults at high

References

[1]  L. Evans, Traffic Safety, Science Serving Society, Bloomfield Hills, Mich, USA, 2004.
[2]  O. J. Clay, V. G. Wadley, J. D. Edwards, D. L. Roth, D. L. Roenker, and K. K. Ball, “Cumulative meta-analysis of the relationship between useful field of view and driving performance in older adults: current and future implications,” Optometry & Vision Science, vol. 82, no. 8, pp. 724–731, 2005.
[3]  L. Hoffman, P. Atchley, J. M. McDowd, and R. Dubinsky, “The role of visual attention in predicting driving impairment in older adults,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 20, no. 4, pp. 610–622, 2005.
[4]  M. Woollacott and A. Shumway-Cook, “Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research,” Gait & Posture, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 1–14, 2002.
[5]  B. Kerr, S. M. Condon, and L. A. McDonald, “Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol. 11, no. 5, pp. 617–622, 1985.
[6]  S. Kemper, R. E. Herman, and C. H. T. Lian, “The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech or noise,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 181–192, 2003.
[7]  H. E. Pashler, The Psychology of Attention, The MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1998.
[8]  C. D. Wickens, “Multiple resources and performance prediction,” Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, vol. 3, no. 2, pp. 159–177, 2002.
[9]  A. F. Kramer and D. Madden, “Attention,” in The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, F. I. M. Craik and T. A. Salthouse, Eds., pp. 189–249, Psychology Press, New York, NY, USA, 3rd edition, 2008.
[10]  J. Kray and U. Lindenberger, “Adult age differences in task switching,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 126–147, 2000.
[11]  P. S. Tsang and T. L. Shaner, “Age, attention, expertise, and time-sharing performance,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 13, no. 2, pp. 323–347, 1998.
[12]  A. F. Kramer, J. Larish, T. Weber, and L. Bardell, “Training for executive control: task coordination strategies and aging,” in Attention and Performance XVII, D. Gopher and A. Koriat, Eds., The MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, USA, 1999.
[13]  U. Lindenberger, M. Marsiske, and P. B. Baltes, “Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 15, no. 3, pp. 417–436, 2000.
[14]  K. Z. H. Li, U. Lindenberger, A. M. Freund, and P. B. Baltes, “Walking while memorizing: age-related differences in compensatory behavior,” Psychological Science, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 230–237, 2001.
[15]  O. Huxhold, S. C. Li, F. Schmiedek, and U. Lindenberger, “Dual-tasking postural control: aging and the effects of cognitive demand in conjunction with focus of attention,” Brain Research Bulletin, vol. 69, no. 3, pp. 294–305, 2006.
[16]  M. B. Neider, J. G. Gaspar, J. S. McCarley, J. A. Crowell, H. Kaczmarski, and A. F. Kramer, “Walking and talking: dual-task effects on street crossing behavior in older adults,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 260–268, 2011.
[17]  M. E. Tinetti, M. Speechley, and S. F. Ginter, “Risk factors for falls among elderly persons living in the community,” The New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 319, no. 26, pp. 1701–1707, 1988.
[18]  A. J. Blake, K. Morgan, M. J. Bendall et al., “Falls by elderly people at home: prevalence and associated factors,” Age and Ageing, vol. 17, no. 6, pp. 365–372, 1988.
[19]  O. Beauchet, V. Dubost, G. Allali, R. Gonthier, F. R. Hermann, and R. W. Kressig, “‘Faster counting while walking’ as a predictor of falls in older adults,” Age and Ageing, vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 418–423, 2007.
[20]  L. Lundin-Olsson, L. Nyberg, and Y. Gustafson, “‘Stops walking when talking’ as a predictor of falls in elderly people,” The Lancet, vol. 349, no. 9052, p. 617, 1997.
[21]  J. Verghese, H. Buschke, L. Viola et al., “Validity of divided attention tasks in predicting falls in older individuals: a preliminary study,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 50, no. 9, pp. 1572–1576, 2002.
[22]  K. A. Faulkner, M. S. Redfern, J. A. Cauley et al., “Multitasking: association between poorer performance and a history of recurrent falls,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 55, no. 4, pp. 570–576, 2007.
[23]  L. S. Nagamatsu, M. Voss, M. B. Neider et al., “Increased cognitive load leads to impaired mobility decisions in seniors at risk for falls,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 26, no. 2, pp. 253–259, 2011.
[24]  J. M. Hausdorff, G. M. Doniger, S. Springer, G. Yogev, N. Giladi, and E. S. Simon, “A common cognitive profile in elderly fallers and in patients with Parkinson's disease: the prominence of impaired executive function and attention,” Experimental Aging Research, vol. 32, no. 4, pp. 411–429, 2006.
[25]  L. J. Rapport, R. A. Hanks, S. R. Millis, and S. A. Deshpande, “Executive functioning and predictors of falls in the rehabilitation setting,” Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, vol. 79, no. 6, pp. 629–633, 1998.
[26]  G. Yogev-Seligmann, J. M. Hausdorff, and N. Giladi, “The role of executive function and attention in gait,” Movement Disorders, vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 329–342, 2008.
[27]  S. Springer, N. Giladi, C. Peretz, G. Yogev, E. S. Simon, and J. M. Hausdorff, “Dual-tasking effects on gait variability: the role of aging, falls, and executive function,” Movement Disorders, vol. 21, no. 7, pp. 950–957, 2006.
[28]  G. Daigneault, P. Joly, and J. Y. Frigon, “Executive functions in the evaluation of accident risk of older drivers,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 24, no. 2, pp. 221–238, 2002.
[29]  J. M. Wood, K. J. Anstey, G. K. Kerr, P. F. Lacherez, and S. Lord, “A multidomain approach for predicting older driver safety under in-traffic road conditions,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 56, no. 6, pp. 986–993, 2008.
[30]  P. A. Hoggarth, C. R. H. Innes, J. C. Dalrymple-Alford, J. E. Severinsen, and R. D. Jones, “Comparison of a linear and a non-linear model for using sensory-motor, cognitive, personality, and demographic data to predict driving ability in healthy older adults,” Accident Analysis and Prevention, vol. 42, no. 6, pp. 1759–1768, 2010.
[31]  C. A. Fischer, G. V. Kondraske, and R. Malcolm Stewart, “Prediction of driving performance using nonlinear causal resource analysis,” in Proceedings of the 2nd Joint Engineering in Medicine and Biology, 24th Annual Conference and the Annual Fall Meeting of the Biomedical Engineering Society EMBS/BMES Conference, pp. 2473–2474, 2002.
[32]  K. L. Margolis, R. P. Kerani, P. McGovern, T. Songer, J. A. Cauley, and K. E. Ensrud, “Risk factors for motor vehicle crashes in older women,” Journals of Gerontology A, vol. 57, no. 3, pp. M186–M191, 2002.
[33]  K. K. Ball, D. L. Roenker, V. G. Wadley et al., “Can high-risk older drivers be identified through performance-based measures in a department of motor vehicles setting?” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 54, no. 1, pp. 77–84, 2006.
[34]  D. Podsiadlo and S. Richardson, “The timed ‘Up & Go’: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 39, no. 2, pp. 142–148, 1991.
[35]  S. R. Lord, H. B. Menz, and A. Tiedemann, “A physiological profile approach to falls risk assessment and prevention,” Physical Therapy, vol. 83, no. 3, pp. 237–252, 2003.
[36]  S. R. Lord and J. Dayhew, “Visual risk factors for falls in older people,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 49, no. 5, pp. 508–515, 2001.
[37]  S. R. Lord, R. D. Clark, and I. W. Webster, “Physiological factors associated with falls in an elderly population,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 39, no. 12, pp. 1194–1200, 1991.
[38]  K. Delbaere, J. C. T. Close, J. Heim et al., “A multifactorial approach to understanding fall risk in older people,” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, vol. 58, no. 9, pp. 1679–1685, 2010.
[39]  E. Becic, G. S. Dell, K. Bock, S. M. Garnsey, T. Kubose, and A. F. Kramer, “Driving impairs talking,” Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 15–21, 2010.
[40]  M. S. Horswill, K. J. Anstey, C. G. Hatherly, and J. M. Wood, “The crash involvement of older drivers is associated with their hazard perception latencies,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 16, no. 5, pp. 939–944, 2010.
[41]  M. S. Horswill, S. A. Marrington, C. M. McCullough et al., “The hazard perception ability of older drivers,” Journals of Gerontology B, vol. 63, no. 4, pp. P212–P218, 2008.
[42]  M. R. E. Romoser and D. L. Fisher, “The effect of active versus passive training strategies on improving older drivers' scanning in intersections,” Human Factors, vol. 51, no. 5, pp. 652–668, 2009.

Full-Text

Contact Us

service@oalib.com

QQ:3279437679

WhatsApp +8615387084133