Growing evidence supports the use of physical training interventions to improve both physical and cognitive performances in healthy older adults. Few studies have examined the impact of aerobic exercise on Stroop task performance, a measure of executive functions. In the current 3-month aerobic training study, 50 older adults (mean age = 67.96 ± 6.25 years) were randomly assigned to either a three-month physical training group or to a control group (waiting list). Training sessions were 3 times per week for 60 minutes. All participants completed pre- and post-test measures of cognitive performance using the modified Stroop task and physical performance (Rockport one-mile test). Compared to controls, the training group showed significant improvements in physical capacity ( ) and enhanced Stroop performance, but only in the inhibition/switching condition ( ). Furthermore, the increase in aerobic capacity induced by the training regimen correlated negatively with reaction time in the inhibition/switching condition of the Stroop task at posttest ( ; ). Importantly, the reported gains in cognitive performance were observed after only three months of physical training. Taken together, the results suggest that even short-term physical interventions can enhance older adults’ executive functions. 1. Introduction A variety of executive function processes sustained by the prefrontal cortex decline over time [1] (e.g., response preparation [2] and task switching [3]). Recent accounts of executive functions suggest that they rely on distinct basic mechanisms [4], namely, updating (of new information), inhibition (suppressing prepotent responses) and shifting (from one mental set to another), that could be differentially altered as we age. The variability in the trajectories of cognitive decline [5] suggests that compensatory mechanisms [6] and individual factors (i.e., involvement in physical activity and cognitive reserve) [7] could minimize these deficits. Physical activity, defined as any activity that involves bodily movements, is one individual factor that can reduce the impact of aging on executive functions [8, 9]. In goal-directed research, physical activity that was planned, structured, and repetitive improves physical fitness, defined as the ability to function efficiently and effectively in work and leisure activities and to meet unforeseen emergency situations [10]. Theoretically, the “selective improvement” hypothesis [11] argues that aerobic exercise known to increase cardiorespiratory fitness as indexed by direct measures or estimations of (i.e.,
References
[1]
O. A. Coubard, L. Ferrufino, M. Boura, A. Gripon, M. Renaud, and L. Bherer, “Attentional control in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease,” Neuropsychology, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 353–367, 2011.
[2]
L. Bherer and S. Belleville, “Age-related differences in response preparation: the role of time uncertainty,” Journals of Gerontology. Series B, vol. 59, no. 2, pp. P66–P74, 2004.
[3]
A. Hampshire, A. Gruszka, S. J. Fallon, and A. M. Owen, “Inefficiency in self-organized attentional switching in the normal aging population is associated with decreased activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 20, no. 9, pp. 1670–1686, 2008.
[4]
A. Miyake, N. P. Friedman, M. J. Emerson, A. H. Witzki, A. Howerter, and T. D. Wager, “The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis,” Cognitive Psychology, vol. 41, no. 1, pp. 49–100, 2000.
[5]
R. L. Buckner, “Memory and executive function in aging and ad: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate,” Neuron, vol. 44, no. 1, pp. 195–208, 2004.
[6]
R. Cabeza, N. D. Anderson, J. K. Locantore, and A. R. McIntosh, “Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults,” NeuroImage, vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 1394–1402, 2002.
[7]
A. M. Tucker and Y. Stern, “Cognitive reserve in aging,” Current Alzheimer Research, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 354–360, 2011.
[8]
F. Sofi, D. Valecchi, D. Bacci et al., “Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies,” Journal of Internal Medicine, vol. 269, no. 1, pp. 107–117, 2011.
[9]
M. W. Voss, L. S. Nagamatsu, T. Liu-Ambrose, and A. F. Kramer, “Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span,” Journal of Applied Physiology, vol. 111, no. 5, pp. 1505–1513, 2011.
[10]
C. J. Caspersen, K. E. Powell, and G. Christenson, “Physical activity, exercise and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research,” Public Health Reports, vol. 100, no. 2, pp. 126–131, 1985.
[11]
A. F. Kramer, S. Hahn, N. J. Cohen et al., “Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function,” Nature, vol. 400, no. 6743, pp. 418–419, 1999.
[12]
S. Colcombe and A. F. Kramer, “Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study,” Psychological Science, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 125–130, 2003.
[13]
M. Renaud, F. Maquestiaux, S. Joncas, M.-J. Kergoat, and L. Bherer, “The effect of three months of aerobic training on response preparation in older adults,” Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 2, 148, 2010.
[14]
T. Liu-Ambrose, L. S. Nagamatsu, M. W. Voss, K. M. Khan, and T. C. Handy, “Resistance training and functional plasticity of the aging brain: a 12-month randomized controlled trial,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 33, no. 8, pp. 1690–1698, 2012.
[15]
J. Barenberg, T. Berse, and S. Dutke, “Executive functions in learning processes: do they benefit from physical activity?” Educational Research Review, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 208–222, 2011.
[16]
T. Harada, S. Okagawa, and K. Kubota, “Jogging improved performance of a behavioral branching task: implications for prefrontal activation,” Neuroscience Research, vol. 49, no. 3, pp. 325–337, 2004.
[17]
H. L. Hawkins, A. F. Kramer, and D. Capaldi, “Aging, exercise, and attention,” Psychology and aging, vol. 7, no. 4, pp. 643–653, 1992.
[18]
C. M. MacLeod, “Half a century of reseach on the stroop effect: an integrative review,” Psychological Bulletin, vol. 109, no. 2, pp. 163–203, 1991.
[19]
N. B. Cohn, R. E. Dustman, and D. C. Bradford, “Age-related decrements in stroop color test performance,” Journal of Clinical Psychology, vol. 40, no. 5, pp. 1244–1250, 1984.
[20]
A. K. Troyer, L. Leach, and E. Strauss, “Aging and response inhibition: normative data for the Victoria Stroop Test,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition B, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 20–35, 2006.
[21]
R. West, “The effects of aging on controlled attention and conflict processing in the stroop task,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 103–113, 2004.
[22]
J. M. Bugg, E. L. DeLosh, D. B. Davalos, and H. P. Davis, “Age differences in stroop interference: contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits,” Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, vol. 14, no. 2, pp. 155–167, 2007.
[23]
N. Bohnen, J. Jolles, and A. Twijnstra, “Modification of the Stroop Color Word Test improves differentiation between patients with mild head injury and matched controls,” Clinical Neuropsychologist, vol. 6, no. 2, pp. 178–184, 1992.
[24]
P. Verhaeghen, “Aging and executive control: reports of a demise greatly exaggerated,” Current Directions in Psychological Science, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 174–180, 2011.
[25]
L. Vaughan and K. Giovanello, “Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 25, no. 2, pp. 343–355, 2010.
[26]
A. Vuillemin, J. M. Oppert, F. Guillemin et al., “Self-administered questionnaire compared with interview to assess past-year physical activity,” Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 32, no. 6, pp. 1119–1124, 2000.
[27]
M. F. Folstein, S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh, “‘Mini mental state’. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 12, no. 3, pp. 189–198, 1975.
[28]
J. A. Yesavage, T. L. Brink, and T. L. Rose, “Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report,” Journal of Psychiatric Research, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 37–49, 1982.
[29]
G. M. Kline, J. P. Porcari, and R. Hintermeister, “Estimation of VO2max from a one-mile track walk, gender, age, and body weight,” Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 253–259, 1987.
[30]
M. E. Nelson, W. J. Rejeski, S. N. Blair et al., “Physical activity and public health in older adults: recommendation from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association,” Circulation, vol. 116, no. 9, pp. 1094–1105, 2007.
[31]
D. R. Hopkins, B. Murrah, W. W. K. Hoeger, and R. C. Rhodes, “Effect of low-impact aerobic dance on the functional fitness of elderly women,” Gerontologist, vol. 30, no. 2, pp. 189–192, 1990.
[32]
O. A. Coubard, S. Duretz, V. Lefebvre, P. Lapalus, and L. Ferrufino, “Practice of contemporary dance improves cognitive flexibility in aging,” Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, vol. 3, no. SEP, pp. 1–12, 2011.
[33]
R. E. Dustman, R. O. Ruhling, and E. M. Russell, “Aerobic exercise training and improved neuropsychological function of older individuals,” Neurobiology of Aging, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 35–42, 1984.
[34]
J. A. Blumenthal, C. F. Emery, D. J. Madden et al., “Cardiovascular and behavioral effects of aerobic exercise training in healthy older men and women,” Journals of Gerontology, vol. 44, no. 5, pp. M147–M157, 1989.
[35]
P. Verhaeghen and L. De Meersman, “Aging and the Stroop effect: a meta-analysis,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 13, no. 1, pp. 120–126, 1998.
[36]
P. Verhaeghen and J. Cerella, “Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, vol. 26, no. 7, pp. 849–857, 2002.
[37]
T. A. Salthouse, “The Processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition,” Psychological Review, vol. 103, no. 3, pp. 403–428, 1996.
[38]
A. L. Smiley-Oyen, K. A. Lowry, S. J. Francois, M. L. Kohut, and P. Ekkekakis, “Exercise, fitness, and neurocognitive function in older adults: the “selective improvement” and “cardiovascular fitness” hypotheses,” Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 36, no. 3, pp. 280–291, 2008.
[39]
I. Lista and G. Sorrentino, “Biological mechanisms of physical activity in preventing cognitive decline,” Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, vol. 30, no. 4, pp. 493–503, 2010.
[40]
A. F. Kramer and K. I. Erickson, “Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 11, no. 8, pp. 342–348, 2007.
[41]
K. I. Erickson, A. M. Weinstein, B. P. Sutton, et al., “Beyond vascularization: aerobic fitness is associated with N-acetylaspartate and working memory,” Brain and Behavior, vol. 2, no. 1, pp. 32–41, 2012.
[42]
S. A. Langenecker, K. A. Nielson, and S. M. Rao, “fMRI of healthy older adults during Stroop interference,” NeuroImage, vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 192–200, 2004.
[43]
M. Lagu?-Beauvais, J. Brunet, L. Gagnon, F. Lesage, and L. Bherer, “A fNIRS investigation of switching and inhibition during the modified Stroop task in younger and older adults,” NeuroImage, vol. 64, pp. 485–495, 2013.
[44]
E. M. Fine, D. C. Delis, S. R. Wetter et al., “Cognitive discrepancies versus apoe genotype as predictors of cognitive decline in normal-functioning elderly individuals: a longitudinal study,” American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, vol. 16, no. 5, pp. 366–374, 2008.
[45]
L. R. Clark, D. M. Schiehser, G. H. Weissberger, D. P. Salmon, D. C. Delis, and M. W. Bondi, “Specific measures of executive function predict cognitive decline in older adults,” Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, vol. 18, no. 1, pp. 118–127, 2012.
[46]
S. R. Wetter, D. C. Delis, W. S. Houston et al., “Deficits in inhibition and flexibility are associated with the APOE-E4 allele in nondemented older adults,” Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, vol. 27, no. 8, pp. 943–952, 2005.
[47]
K. A. Hutchison, D. A. Balota, and J. M. Ducheck, “The utility of stroop task switching as a marker for early-stage alzheimer's disease,” Psychology and Aging, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 545–559, 2010.